Madrid, D. (2000): “Learning Strategies”,
en Teaching English as a Foreign Language, Barcelona: The Australian
Institute.
LANGUAGE LEARNING STRATEGIES
(University of Granada)
One
of the many consequences of the communicative language approach in language
teaching and learning has been the enhancement of the role of the learner in the language learning process. The old
belief that the teacher teaches and learners learn what they are taught is not
maintained nowadays, not only based on direct observation but also on research,
which has repeatedly proved that learners regularly don’t learn what teachers
teach. This phenomenon have contributed to focus our attention from the teacher
to the learner, from the past focus on the improvement of teaching to an
increased concern for how learners go about their learning tasks and how the
process of learning takes place.
On
the other hand, the traditional curriculum distinctions between content
and methodology is not so pronounced and the emphasis on curricular contents
(what the students learn) has moved on to the process of
learning (how and why
they learn), which is part of the methodological stage. Content and
methodology, not only play a different role in today´s teaching and learning
situations, the process of language learning has become part of the content
of learning. This focus on
learning and on the learner makes the
classroom dynamics and the curriculum implementation very different from the
past: the whole process is much more learner-centred and the students become
more responsible about their learning than they were in the past. This means
that they have to be more conscious about their individual learning processes
and the strategies that they use in each learning act; that is, they are
supposed to learn how to learn in order to know how they learn more
efficiently.
But
this awareness about the learning processes cannot be taken for granted. It may
be latent and at times it may work in a subconscious way, but it needs
training. Awareness is not enough, learners need awareness with a purpose. The
learners´ co-responsibility in the learning process implies reflecting and
learning about themselves and knowing how to act autonomously as
learners in each teaching and learning situation. It is this complex
relationship between strategy and autonomy, learning to learn and learner’s responsibility in the learning
process what is crucial in this unit.
So,
this module is concerned with learning strategies (LS). It focuses on
the application of LS to second language learning (SLL) and/or second language
acquisition (SLA) by students learning English (or any other language) as a
second (L2) or foreign language (FL). The different sections that we will
introduce here describe the role that LS play in the SLL/SLA processes. We will
follow some of the major works on the
topic (Rubin, 1975; Naiman et al. 1978; Chesterfield and Chesterfield, 1985;
O’Malley and Chamot, 1990; Oxford, 1990; Wenden, 1991; Jiménez Raya, 1993;
Manchón and Bruton, 1993; Beltrán, 1993 and Valcárcel, Coyle y Verdú, 1996).
On
the other hand, we will base most of our work on the cognitive theory of
learning as developed in various contributions by Lanchman, Lanchman and
Butterfield, (1979); Gagné, (1985); Shuell, (1986); Weinstein and Mayer, (1986)
and, above all, Anderson, (1980, 1983 and 1985) and O’Malley and Chamot,
(1990).
Do
learners know the aspects or components of each English unit, what each section
aims to, the skills they are developing? Read and match these sentences (from
Madrid, D. and McLaren, N. (1997):
Making Progress 3, Workbook, (p. 5). Valladolid:
Ed. La Calesa.):
What components or sections do learners and
teachers find most useful to learn the language? How much do learners learn when doing the following? (from Madrid, D. and McLaren, N.
(1997): Making Progress 1, Workbook, (p. 6). Valladolid: Ed. La
Calesa.):
___________________________________________________________________________
2. A
COGNITIVE THEORY OF LEARNING
Linguistic
theories have long assumed that language is learnt separately from cognitive
skills, according to different principles.
Language and linguistic processes are viewed as interacting with
cognition but they maintain a separate identity. One of the principal cognitive
processes that has not been taken into account in these theories is learning
strategies. But more recently, some researchers have turned away from
linguistic based theories towards the field of cognitive psychology in their
attempts to explain the processes involved in SLL and SLA. Cognitive approaches
to SLA consider that language is no longer seen as a unique and separate form
of knowledge but as a complex cognitive skill that can be described within the context
of how people acquire and store knowledge in general. O'Malley and Chamot have
argued that SLA cannot be fully understood without taking into account the
interaction between language and cognition in the storage of information in
memory and in the acquisition of new information.
The role of LS in the acquisition of
information can be understood by reference to the information processing
framework for learning. The purpose of this framework is to explain how
information is stored in the memory and how new information is acquired. In its
simplest form, the framework suggests that information is stored in two
distinct ways, either in short-term memory or long-term memory.
The short-term memory (STM)
If
we lose our short term memory, as a result of a head injury in a motorbike
accident, we can tell the doctor
details about –let’s say our infancy- but if asked where we have left our motorbike, for example, we
would reply: 'What motorbike are you talking about?' That is, the accident has
damaged our short term memory (STM). Sometimes, of course, the contents of the
STM are passed on to the long term memory (LTM) where they are structured. In
order to pass into the long term memory, information must first be processed
and structured in the short term memory so that it 'makes sense' to the
student. The process of structuring new information takes time; but it is time
well spent, because students find it almost impossible to remember something
that they do not properly understand. This vital process of structuring or
giving meaning to new information is demanding as well as time-consuming, so we
must try to give our students as much help as we can. Learning activities that
involve students in using the new ideas will aid clarification. This
search for structure also explains why many learners appreciate being given summaries
and well-organised notes.
The long-term memory (LTM)
Once the STM has 'made
sense' of the information, it is then passed into the long term memory (LTM)
where, unless it is subsequently used or recalled in some other way, it is
again eventually forgotten. Forgetting and remembering, then, are not under
direct conscious control; they are automatic. There is only one way to ensure
that something is remembered: repetition and practice. As teachers, we
must make sure that any knowledge we want our students to remember is recalled
and used frequently. Watson, the father of the behaviourist school of
psychology developed by Skinner, admitted this when he said that remembering
depended on 'frequency and occurrence'.
According to Weinstein
and Mayer (1986), in this cognitive psychology paradigm, new information is
acquired through a four-stage encoding process involving selection,
acquisition, construction and integration (O’Malley and Chamot,
1990: 17-18):
- Selection: in this stage learners focus on
specific information of interest in the environment and transfer that
information into working memory.
- Acquisition: later, learners
actively transfer information from working memory into long-term-memory for
permanent storage.
- Construction: thirdly, learners build
internal connections between ideas contained in working memory, this
information can be used to enrich the learner’s understanding or retentionof
the new ideas.
- Integration: in this final process,
the learner searcher for prior knowledge in long-term memory and transfers this
knowledge to working memory.
Selection and acquisition determine how much is learned, whereas construction
and integration determine what is learned and how it is
organized.
Nevertheless, other
cognitive models of learning propose a
different sequence which includes (Madrid et al. 1998):
-
selecting information from the environment,
-
organizing
the information,
-
relating
it to what we
already know,
-
retaining what we consider to be important,
-
using
the information in
appropriate contexts and situations,
-
reflecting
on the success of
the learning efforts and evaluating the effectiveness of results.
Among the different alternatives and
approaches to linguistic theories O'Malley and Chamot (1990) base most of their
discussion on John Anderson's information processing model of cognitive skill
learning (1980, 1983, 1985) for five reasons (1990:19):
- Anderson’s
work integrates numerous concepts that give the theory generality.
- It covers a
broad range behaviour other than theories: comprehension and production of oral
and written texts, problem solving, etc.
- The theory
distinguishes between factual knowledge and procedural skills.
- It
incorporates strategic processing.
- It has been
continually updated, expanded and revised.
2.1. Representation in memory
The
representation of knowledge in memory is a key concept in Anderson's theory. He
makes an important distinction between 'static' information or knowing about
something, which is referred to as declarative knowledge and 'dynamic'
information or knowing how to do something, which he terms procedural
knowledge.
Declarative
or factual knowledge.
It is stored in long term memory in the form of cognitive units of meaning such
as:
a) Propositional networks: associations of meaning between
important elements in a sentence. Its basic unit is the node (similar to
what people call ideas) and the connection between nodes are links.
b) Schema: larger units of interconnected features which define
a concept. Schemata may be composed of propositional neworks. Their principal
value is that they facilitate making inferences about concepts.
Procedural
knowledge refers to the ability to
understand and generate language or apply our knowledge of rules to solve a
problem or carry out a particular skill, such as for example, riding a bicycle
or playing the piano. It refers to the processes involved in learning how to do
something successfully. In terms of language acquisition procedural knowledge
is seen as our ability to understand and produce language (see Valcárcel, Coyle
and Verdú, 1996). The representation of procedural knowledge in memory is a key
issue in cognitive theory and is contained in what Anderson refers to production
systems.
-
What’s the difference between STM
and LTM?
-
Give some teaching
recommendations to favour the development of LTM.
-
What’s the difference between
declarative and procedural knowledge?
-
What are the components of
declarative knowledge?
-
What kind of knowledge do we
develop when we teach grammar rules?
-
What type of knowledge does the
leaner develop when (s)he writes a composition? Why?
___________________________________________________________________________
3.
COGNITIVE THEORY APPLIED TO SECOND LANGUAGE ACQUISITION
The
distinction made by Anderson between declarative and procedural
knowledge has obvious implications for both the theory and practice of SLA. Faerch and Kasper (1985, 1987) were the
first to apply this concept to the field of SLA. They stated that the learner's
declarative knowledge consisted of internalized interlanguage rules and
memorized chunks of language whereas procedural knowledge were those strategies
and procedures used by the learner to process L2 information for acquisition
and use. According to Faerch and
Kasper, procedural knowledge can be differenciated into five separate
components (O’Malley and Chamot, 1990:58-59):
1)
Reception
procedures, such as the use of inferencing to extrapolate meaning.
2)
Production
procedures, such as
planning and monitoring speech production.
3)
Conversational
procedures, such as
following linguistic principles that produce coherent texts.
4)
Communication
strategies, which are intended to solve
problems in speech comprehension.
5)
Learning
procedures, such as the
development of interlanguage kowledge through hypothesis formation and testing.
As Faerch and Kasper suggest, most
declarative knowledge is activated in a conscious manner, while procedural
knowledge tends to be more automatic and is activated without awareness, except
when the language user has interruptions in communication.
Conscious
awareness
As
Valcárcel et al. (1996) have argued, the idea that simply knowing 'about' the
language is insufficient if what the learner wants is to be able to use the
language for successful communication. To use the L2 functionally the learner
must have acquired the necessary procedural knowledge, which, in Anderson's
terms, can only be mastered slowly and after a great deal of practice. With
this in mind it becomes clear that L2 teachers need to concentrate on providing
learners with communicative activities which focus on language as the
acquisition of a skill rather than as an object of study (see also Martínez and
Valcárcel, 1992, 1993).
The
distinction between declarative and procedural knowledge has also
important implications for the concept of language transfer. The
transfer of knowledge from one language to another refers not only to
declarative knowledge but also to the proceduralized knowledge involved in
language comprehension and production. When processes are learned successfully
in the L1 the learner is able to transfer them to similar situations in the L2.
On the other hand, concepts related to natural categories such as science,
mathematics, and technical subjects may be easier to transfer to the L2 than
concepts related to culturally affected areas such as literature o social
studies. This theory of the representation of meaning in memory is consistent
with the notion of language transfer in SLA as an active learner strategy and
an aid to acquisition (1996:83-84)
Stages
of skill acquisition
The
important question that follows from the distinction between declarative and
procedural knowledge is how the mind proceeds from rule-bound declarative
knowledge used in performance of a complex skill to the more automatic
proceduralized stage? Anderson
described three stages in this process of skill acquisition: the cognitive,
the associative and the autonomous (see O’Malley y Chamot,
1990:25-27):
a)
Cognitive stage
Anderson
describes the acquisition of a complex
skill in terms of the proceduralization of declarative knowledge. Applying this
to SLA O'Malley and Chamot suggest that during the cognitive stage the
L2 learner engages in conscious and intensive mental activity in order to make
sense of the new language. In natural settings this would involve using the
language functionally although the learner may not fully understand the
underlying structures of the expressions used. In classroom settings the
learner would pay deliberate attention to the formal structure of the language
or to the use of chunks and formulas in
communicative activities. The principle characteristic of this first stage in
the learning process is the concentrated attention paid to the new language
forms in order to find meaning.
In
the associative stage learners begin to use their previously acquired
knowledge procedurally. The L2 is used for communicative purposes although
errors can still be detected in learner speech. The learner continues, however,
to have difficulty in using new L2 information as attention in this second
stage, now directed at improving the language skill, reduces the amount of
conscious effort available for transforming new input into declarative
knowledge.
c)
Autonomous stage
In
the third and final autonomous stage, performance in the L2 resembles
closely that of a native speaker. The learner uses the L2 fluently and without
reference to linguistic rules. Language processing has become autonomous and
acquisition of the skill accomplished.
Use a textbook and select three activities, each one
related to the 3 stages established by O’Mally and Chamot:
a)
Cognitive stage
b)
Associative stage
c)
Autonomous stage
___________________________________________________________________________
4.1.
Learning by formal rules
As O'Malley and Chamot have pointed out
(1990:27-31), the problem with Anderson’s theory, however, lies in the
assumption that declarative knowledge for SLA consists essentially of the
formal rules of the language. To accept Anderson's theory it is necessary to
extend his definition of declarative knowledge to include not only the
grammatical rules of the L2, which may or may not be taught explicitly in the
second language classroom, but also those highly individual 'rules' which
learners produce as a result of their own learning experience which reflect the
imperfect and temporary form their interlanguage. The fact that skill
acquisition begins with the learning of
the formal rules of the language has
also been criticized on the grounds that it could lead to an inefficient and
tiresome teaching methodology (see Valcárcel et al. 1996:84-85). In the
acquisition of an L2 learners prefer to become actively involved in performing
the skill as early as possible in the learning process whether they are
familiar with the formal rules of the language or not. A classroom approach
which focused solely on learning grammatical rules before allowing the learner
to attempt to perform the skill would soon prove frustrating.
Another
problem with Anderson’s theory is the insistence on a single process to explain
all forms of learning complex cognitive skills. As Rumelhart and Norman (1978)
suggest, the learning of a complex cognitive skill may entail other processes.
They distinguish three stages:
a)
Restructuring: it includes the development of novel structures
for interpreting new information and for reorganizing existing knowledge.
b)
Accretion: gradual accumulation of new information by
matching by matching new data to existing schemata.
c)
Tuning: it acts to refine the existing knowledge
based on modification of available knowledge structures.
But
the point is whether restructuring, accretion and tuning represent unique forms
of learning or can be represented through the stages described by Anderson
(cognitive, associative, autonomous).
Another
possible limitation of Anderson’s theory of learning complex cognitive skills
is that it may lead to inefficient instructional approaches. As Gagné (1985)
notes, requiring students to learn rules as declarative knowledge before they
can perform the steps in a complex skill is a tedious way to learn. A more
effective method for learning a complex skill would be to model the performance
required by the learner while providing opportunities for practising the
components of the skill until they become automatic. In the communicative
language classroom the teacher would model the use of the L2 and provide
feedback to encourage meaningful communication.
Faerch
and Kasper (1985) have also proposed learning through imitation as one of the
basic processes in the acquisition and automatization of a second language.
They suggest that unanalysed chunks of language acquired through imitation are
stored in short term memory where they are gradually combined and
proceduralized for automatic use. A second process described in their work
involves hypothesis formation and testing in which learners form hypotheses
based on their previous L1 or L2 knowledge and test them out in comparison with
L2 input acquired receptively, productively, metalingually or interactionally
(see also Valcárcel et al. 1996). Learners are said to test hypothesis in at
least one of four ways (O’Malley and Chamot, 1990:33):
1)
Receptively, by comparing hypotheses to second language
input.
2)
Productively, by using the hypothesis to generate language
and assessing the feedback.
3)
Metalingually, by consulting a native speaker or text.
4)
Interactionally, by making an intentional error to elicit a
repair from a native speaker.
The application of cognitive theory
to SLA research is a relatively recent development and must be evaluated as
such. The theory described here provides an interesting framework for the
description of SLA as that of the acquisition of a complex cognitive skill and
offers insights into such second language constructs as language transfer,
interlanguage and the acquisition-learning distinction and the role played by
learning strategies. However, assumptions made about the language learning
process still require much empirical investigation if cognitive theory is to
provide a valid alternative to the approaches of linguistic based theories.
Helping the learners to discover they learning style
and the way they learn. Read and grade yourself (as an English learner) (from
Madrid, D. and McLaren, N. (1997):
Making Progress 3, Workbook, (p. 12). Valladolid:
Ed. La Calesa.):
5.
LEARNING STRATEGIES AS COGNITIVE SKILLS
As
we have anticipated earlier, Anderson does not distinguish LS from other
cognitive processes, perhaps because his theory focuses not only on LS but
fundamentally on describing how information is stored and retrieved. In
Anderson’s theory, strategies can be represented the same way as any other
complex skill and described as a set of actions that are fine-tuned until they
become procedural knowledge.
One
of the basic principles of the cognitive theory just outlined is the idea that
learners do not merely acquire knowledge but rather that they construct it by
using their previous experience to understand and shape new information. The
role of the teacher is no longer to simply supply this information, but to take
an active part in the process of knowledge formation together with the learner.
Knowledge, in other words, is constructed and shared. This means that
instruction is not transformed directly into output, but that individuals build
up their knowledge actively and meaningfully through the activation of mental
processes. The cognitive theory that is based on these assumptions is called constructivism. According to
this view of learning individuals make their
personal construction from the
information they receive and develop a certain degree of intellectual autonomy.
Students are seen as active co-constructors of knowledge. Constructivism provides a rationale for teaching by negotiation. Teachers foster
the development of higher-order thinking skills through challenging questions,
modelling the learning process, and engaging in interactive dialogue with
students. This form of learning is more demanding for the teacher, who should
have not only a deep understanding of the subject matter but also the ability
to connect it with the students’ cognitive network: previous concepts, experiences and schema. This requires
that the teacher transcend the transmission view of communication derived from
the behaviorist cognitive network theories and apply the principles of cognitive psychology (see Madrid et al., 1998)
As Valcárcel et al. (1996)
have argued, for learning to take place the learner must actively participate
in the process. However, participating does not mean simply listening to the
teacher or looking at the textbook. Taking part in the process of learning
requires the activation and regulation of many additional factors such as:
-
motivation,
-
beliefs about learning,
-
previous knowledge,
-
interaction,
-
new information,
-
abilities and strategies.
Learning is considered as a
process of processes; this means that the acquisition of knowledge involves the
activation of certain mental activities which must be adequately planned in
order to fulfil initial expectations. However, not all learners know or possess
these learning processes, or they use them inadequately, which could
lead to poor learning or no learning at all. For this reason the field of
cognitive psychology, in recent years, has tried to identify those cognitive
processes employed by subjects as they learn, and they have become the major
focus of educational innovation in schools. The
identification of these processes is what will
permit the setting up of programmes of educational improvement and
intervention. This is the true interpretation of what is meant by improving the
quality of learning and here is where the role of strategies becomes
important.
If the nucleus of learning
consists of those processes which act as mediators between instructional and
informative input and the output of the learner, then the nature of learning,
especially in what refers to the quantitative or qualitative nature of
learning, will be determined by those processes or strategies which
are put into practice. In this way learning depends on
-
what the learner does,
-
the processes he uses when learning,
-
the strategies which develop these
processes .
Making the
learners more aware of the strategies and skills developed when receiving oral
input (the listening comprehension skill).
Answer the
following questions after you have listened to a short passage (from Madrid, D.
and McLaren, N. (1997): Making
Progress 3, (p. 26). Valladolid: Ed. La Calesa.):
___________________________________________________________________________
In
this section we will provide several definitions of learning strategies (LS)
and we will describe them within the framework of Anderson’s cognitive theory.
Some of the most popular LS definitions and the theorists who propose them are
presented in the following table:
Authors |
LS definitions |
Rubin (1975, 1987) |
Techniques
or devices which a learner may use to acquire second language knowledge. “What learners do to learn and do to regulate their
learning” (1987:19) |
O’Malley and Chamot (1990) |
Learning strategies are “the special
thoughts or behaviours that individuals use to help them comprehend, learn,
or retain new information”. |
Weinstein and Mayer (1986) |
“the way in which the learner selects, acquirers,
organizes, or integrates new knowledge”. |
Tarone (1981) |
LS are “attempts to develop linguistic and
sociolinguistic competence in the target language” |
Oxford (1990) |
“... operations employed by the learner to aid the
acquisition, storage, retrieval, and use of information”. “Learning
strategies are specific actions taken
by the learner to make learning
easier, faster, more enjoyable, more self-directed, more effective, and more
transferrable to new situations”. |
Dansereau (1985) |
special thoughts or behaviours that learners use to
help them comprehend, retain and/or use the information |
Stern (1992) |
Learning Strategy: learners engage in
activities to achieve certain goals, ... a choice of procedures, ... some
form of long term planning. |
Other
alternative terms used for LS are:
tactics,
techniques, potentially conscious plans, consciously employed operations,
learning skills, functional skills, cognitive abilities, processing strategies,
problem-solving procedures, basic skills,
The general features of language learning
strategies, according to Oxford, are the following (1990:9-13):
1.
Contribute to
the main goal: communicative competence. For example, metacognitive strategies help learners
to regulate their cognition and to focus, plan and evaluate their progress.
Affective strategies develop self-confidence and perseverance needed to become
involved in language learning situations. Social strategies increase
interaction and empathy in
communication.
2.
Allow
learners to become more self-directed. Learners do not need to have the teacher around to
guide them all the time. They are trained to rely more on themselves and be
more responsible for their learning. They are expected to gain more confidence,
involvement and proficiency.
3.
Expand
the role of teachers.
The traditional roles of teachers as authority figures, managers and directors
of learning, leaders, controllers and evaluators are changed into a new
direction to leave space to a new teacher who acts as facilitator, helper,
guide, consultant, adviser and co-communicator.
4.
Are
problem-oriented. LS
are tools which are used because there’s a problem to solve, a task to
accomplish, an objective to meet.
5.
Are
specific actions taken by the learner. LS are specific actions or behaviours accomplished by
the students to enhance their learning. Examples of these actions are: taking
notes, planning for a language task, self-evaluating, etc.
6.
Involve
many aspects of the learner, not just the cognitive. LS are not restricted to cognitive functions.
They also include metacognitive functions like planning, evaluating, and
arranging one’s own learning, and emotional and affective functions as well.
7.
Support
learning both directly and indirectly. Some LS involve direct learning, but others like
metacognitive, social or affective strategies have an indirect effect.
8.
Are
not always observable.
Some LS are not observable to the human eye. For example, the act of making
mental associations cannot be seen. So, we need the learner’s cooperation to
explore the non-observable LS.
9.
Are
often conscious. LS are often
conscious, but as Oxford suggests, after a certain amount of practice and use
they may act in a automatic or subconscious way.
10.
Can
be taught. Another important hypothesis
stated by Oxford is that LS are easy to teach and modify through strategy
training. This training is most effective when students learn why and when
specific strategies are important, how to use these strategies and how to transfer
them to new situations.
11.
Are
flexible. LS are not always found in
predictable sequences. There is a great deal of individuality in the way
learners choose, combine and sequence strategies.
12.
Are
influenced by a variety of factors. Many factors affect the choice of strategies: degree
of awareness, stage of leaning, teacher expectations, age, sex, general
learning style, personality traits, motivation level, etc.
Some authors
have proved that learning strategies develop with age, are used with increasing
sophistication by older students, result in improved task performance and can
be taught (Oxford, 1990; O’Malley and Chamot (1990).
6.1. Processes, strategies and techniques
As
Valcárcel et. al (1996:87) have noted, in the available research literature
there is no consensus of opinion as to the number or indeed the names
attributed to these learning processes and strategies. As Ellis (1985: 166)
points out 'the metalanguage involved in the cognitive components of procedural
knowledge is often confusing and vague'. Researchers do not use terms like
process and strategy consistently. Sometimes they are used as
synonyms for general mental operations and at other times to differentiate
operations involved in language processing.
A widely
accepted definition of process and strategy is that put forward by Faerch and
Kasper (1980) according to whom a process implies a sequence of
operations in the development of a plan, as in reception or production
processes, and a strategy is defined as a single operation or feature of
that process (see LS taxonomy by Valcárcel et al.) Processes constitute the
goals of the various learning strategies. In this way a learner classifies
semantic elements in order to retain them in memory and store this new
knowledge. Strategies consist of behaviours or concrete mental operations
related to a specific goal which are carried out by students at the moment of
learning. These behaviours are observable, either directly or indirectly,
during the learning process.
While
the processes involved in learning are invisible and as such are difficult to
evaluate and train, the strategies which activate them are more visible and
more susceptible to teaching and training. To supply and promote the
development of learning strategies in students is not a question of teaching
them new content, but rather of training them in the acquisition of a skill
which once learnt can be transferred to other situations, facilitating in this
way the learning process. It is, in effect, a question of learning to learn.
Learning is no longer restricted to the acquisition of content (declarative
knowledge) but to the acquisition of skills (procedural knowledge)
with which to learn this content.
Given
the lack of agreement among theorists and researchers, Valcárcel et al. (1996)
have opted for the following terminology:
-
Processes are a general category of actions directed at
the acquisition and transfer of information.
-
Strategies are defined as the special actions or
behaviours that learners use to help them learn.
-
Tactic or technique is used to refer to
specific learner activities.
Techniques can be said to activate strategies in
that they are observable behaviours which reveal the presence of particular strategies in learners.
Strategies play a mediating role between processes and techniques and set out
to develop a particular learning process for which specific techniques are
employed (Valcárcel et al. 1996: 86-87).
6.2. Strategies for SLL and SLA
When
we revise the bibliography on SLL/SLA strategies we find an important lack of
agreement in terminology which can be misleading. As Oxford (1990: 17) puts it
'there is no complete agreement on exactly what strategies are; how many exist;
how they should be defined, demarcated and categorized; and whether it is
possible to create a real, scientifically validated hierarchy of strategies'.
As Valcárcel et al. (1996) have argued, one cause for that confusion is mainly
due to the fact that the research findings depend on the areas of study, age of
subjects investigated, research data techniques, etc. Consequently, unless the
SLA/FLA strategy paradigm finds a consistent conceptual framework in which
linguistic acquisition, language learning in classroom settings, and cognitive
psychology are combined and their scope limited, LA strategy researchers will
continue to offer biased taxonomies of language learning strategies. The
following sections will offer some brief comments on some of those studies most
often referred to in the literature on learning strategies.
TASK 6
Helping
the learner to become aware of the skills and strategies involved in the
speaking skill. (from Madrid, D. and McLaren, N. (1997): Making Progress 1, (p. 38). Valladolid: Ed. La Calesa.):
-
Now design a similar activity
which makes the learners aware of the skills and strategies involved in grammar
activities. You can go over the examples in the appendix and take ideas.
-
Now let’s see how useful are the
speaking activities to learn and how often are they practised in class? (from
Madrid, D. and McLaren, N. (1997):
Making Progress 3, Workbook, (p. 38 ). Valladolid: Ed. La Calesa.):
___________________________________________________________________________
6.3. Studies in learning strategies
The
literature on learning strategies in SLA emerged from a concerned for
identifying the characteristics of effective learners. Research efforts
concentrating on the good language learner (Rubin 1975; Naiman et al. 1978)
have identified strategies reported by students that appear to contribute to
learning. The list of strategies identified by Rubin and Thompson (1982) can be
summarized under these statements. Effective learners ...
-
find their own
way,
-
organize,
-
are creative,
-
make their own
opportunities,
-
learn to live
with uncertainty,
-
use mnemonics,
-
make errors
work,
-
use their
linguistic knowledge,
-
let context
help them,
-
learn to make
intelligent guesses,
-
learn some
lines as wholes,
-
learn
formalized routines,
-
learn
production techniques,
-
use different
styles of speech.
TASK 7
Exploring the learners opinion
about their efficiency in using specific reading skills and strategies (from
Madrid, D. and McLaren, N. (1997):
Making Progress 3, Workbook, (p. 46 ). Valladolid: Ed. La Calesa.):
Helping the
learners to become more efficient in learning written English (from Madrid, D.
and McLaren, N. (1997): Making
Progress 2, (p. 78 ). Valladolid:
Ed. La Calesa.) and (from Madrid, D. and McLaren, N. (1997): Making Progress 1, Workbook, (p. 42 ). Valladolid: Ed. La Calesa.):
6.3.1. Rubin’s taxonomy
It
seems that it was Rubin (1975) who first introduced the idea that the behaviour
of successful language learners might be different in some way from that of
others. This early work anticipated that competent individuals are effective
because of special ways of processing information. There was also the
suggestion that these could be learned by others who had not discovered them on
their own. Rubin's research mostly used young adults as subjects, students who
happened to be taking ESL classes at the University of Hawaii. Based on this
research, Rubin proposed a classification scheme that includes learning
strategies under two primary groupings which she calls Primary strategies,
within which she includes one group of 'Strategies that directly affect
learning', and another group which includes 'Processes that contribute
indirectly to learning':
Strategies
that directly affect learning (Rubin, 1975) |
- Clarification/verification: Asks for an example of how to use a word or
expression, repeats words to confirm understanding - Monitoring: Corrects errors in own/other's pronunciation, vocabulary, spelling,
grammar, style. - Memorization:Takes notes of new items, pronounces out loud, finds a mnemonic,
writes items repeatedly. - Guessing/inductive inferencing: Guesses meaning from key words,
structures, pictures, context, etc. - Deductive reasoning: Compares native/other language to target language;
Groups words; Looks for rule of co-occurrence - Practice: Experiments with new sounds; Repeats sentences until
pronounced easily; Listens carefully and tries to imitate. |
Processes that contribute
indirectly to learning
|
- Creates opportunities for practice: Creates situation with native speaker;
Initiates conversation with fellow students; Spends time in language
lab, listening to TV, etc. - Production tricks: Uses circumlocutions, synonyms, or cognates; Uses formulaic
interaction; Contextualizes to clarify meaning |
Efficiency of group work (from
Madrid, D. and McLaren, N. (1997):
Making Progress 1, Workbook, (p. 66 ). Valladolid: Ed. La Calesa.):
6.3.2. Naiman,
Frölich, Stern and Todesco’s contribution
Naiman
et al. (1978) proposed an alternative classification scheme which contains five
broad categories of learning strategies and a number of secondary categories.
The primary strategies were found to be common to all good language learners
interviewed, whereas the secondary strategies were represented only in some of
the good learners:
Primary strategy classification and representative
secondary strategies (Naiman et al. 1978) |
- Active task approach: 1.
Responds positively to learning opportunity or seeks and exploits
learning environments. 2.
Adds related language learning activities to regular clasroom program. 3.
Practices - Realization of language as a system: 1.
Analyzes individual problems 2.
Makes L1/L2 comparison 3.
Analyzes target language to make inferences 4.
Makes use of fact that language is a system - Realization of language as a means of
communication and interaction: 1.
Emphasizes fluency over accuracy 2.
Seeks communicative situations with L2 speakers - Management of affective demands: 1.
Finds sociocultural meanings 2.
Copes with affective demands in learning - Monitoring L2 performance: 1.
Constantly revises L2 system by testing inferences and asking L2 native speakers for feedback. |
Naiman
et al. (1978) also identified what they referred to as "techniques"
for second language learning, which differed from strategies in their scheme by
being focused on specific aspects of language learning. The techniques, with
selected examples of each, are as follows (see Valcárcel et al. 1996: 90):
Sound acquisition: repeating aloud after a teacher, a native speaker,
or a tape; listening carefully; and talking aloud, including role playing.
Grammar: Following rules given in texts; inferring grammar
rules from texts; comparing L1 and L2; and memorizing structures and using them
often.
Vocabulary: making up charts and memorizing them; learning words
in context; learning words that are associated; using new words in phrases;
using a dictionary when necessary; and carrying a notebook to note down new
items.
Listening comprehension: listening to the radio, records, TV, movies,
tapes, etc.; and exposing oneself to different accents and registers.
Learning to talk: not being afraid to make mistakes; making contact
with native speakers; asking for corrections; and memorizing dialogues.
Learning to write: having pen pals; writing frequently; and frequent
reading of what you expect to write.
Learning to read: reading something every day; reading things that are
familiar; reading texts at the beginner's level; and looking for meaning from
context without consulting a dictionary.
Read the techniques proposed
by Naiman et al. (1978) again and relate them to the information provided by
the learner in connection with the way they proceed when learning vocabulary
(from Madrid, D. and McLaren, N. (1997):
Making Progress 2, (p. 38). Valladolid:
Ed. La Calesa.):
___________________________________________________________________________
As
Valcárcel et al. (1996) have noticed, although the Rubin (1975) and Naiman et
al. (1978) classification schemes are substantially different, and do not have
any grounding in theories of second language acquisition or cognition, and
their studies do not specify which strategies are fundamental to learning and
which ones might be most useful, they did, however serve as a basis for further
research. Although, not explicitly, they give recognizition to metacognitive
and social strategies. In fact many teachers will see their own classroom
practices reflected in this early research and will be able to recognize the
techniques and strategies used by 'good language learners'.
6.3.3. O’Malley and Chamot’s studies (1990)
One of the best known research work on SL
learning strategies was conducted in the 1980s by O'Malley, Chamot,
Stewner-Manzanares, Kupper and Russo (1985) in the United States, with an
explicit theoretical background in cognitive theory. In a first study, O'Malley
et al. (1985a) collected strategy data on the basis of interviews with
secondary-school ESL learners, interviews with their teachers and observations
and from situations outside the classroom. In this study they reported 26
strategies identified (O'Malley & Chamot 1990: 119). After completing their initial study with
ESL students, O'Malley et al. extended
their research to students studying a foreign language in high school and
college settings to determine if the strategies discovered in ESL students
would be the same as those used by native English-speaking students learning a
foreign language. The results of these studies provided a refinement of their
previous definitions of learning strategies, an analysis of strategic
differences between effective and less effective students, longitudinal
comparisons of students, and a description of preferred strategies for
different types of foreign language tasks. This is the final taxonomy after
their foreign language longitudinal study (O’Malley and Chamot:1990:137-139)
A.
METACOGNITIVE STRATEGIES: They
involve thinking about the learning process, planning for learning,
monitoring the learning task, and evaluating how well one has learned. |
1. Planning: Previewing the organizing concept or principle of an anticipated
learning task (advance organization); proposing strategies for handling an
upcoming task; generating a plan for the parts, sequence, main ideas, or
language functions to be used in handling a task. 2. Directed attention: Deciding in advance to attend in general to a learning task and to
ignore irrelevant distractors; maintaining attention during task execution. 3. Selective attention: Deciding in advance to attend to specific aspects of language input
or situational details to assist in performance of a task; attending to
specific aspects of language input during task execution. 4. Self-management: Understanding the conditions that help one successfully accomplish
language tasks and arranging for the presence of those conditions;
controlling one's language performance to maximize use of what is already
known. 5. Self-monitoring: Checking, verifying, or correcting one's comprehension or
performance in the course of language task. 6. Problem identification: Explicitily identifying the central points needing
resolution in a task or identifying an aspect of the task that hinders its
succesful completion. 7. Self-evaluation: Checking the outcomes of one's own language performance against an
internal measure of completeness and accuracy; checking one's language
repertoire, strategy use, or ability to perform the task at hand. |
B. COGNITIVE STRATEGIES: They involve interacting with the material to be
learned, manipulating the material mentally or physically, or applying a
specific technique to a learning task. |
1. Repetition: Imitating a language model, including overt practice and silent
rehearsal. 2. Resourcing: Using available reference sources of information about the target
language, including dictionaries, textbooks, and prior work. 3. Grouping: Ordering, classifying, or labelling material used in a language task
based on common attributes; recalling information based on grouping
previously done. 4. Note taking: Writing down key words and concepts in abbreviated verbal, graphic,
or numerical form to assist performance of language task. 5. Deduction/Induction: Conciously applying learned or self-developed rules to produce or
understand the target language. 6. Substitution: Selecting alternative approaches, revised plans, or different words
or phrases to accomplish a language task. 7. Elaboration: Relating new information to prior knowledge; relating different
parts of new informa- tion to each other; making meaningful personal
associations to information presented, in the following ways: a.
Personal elaborations: making
judgements about or reacting personally to the material presented. b.
World elaboration: Using
knowledge gained from experience in the world. c.
Academic elaboration: Using
knowledge gained in academic situations. d.
Between parts elaboration:
Relating parts of the task to each other. e.
Question elaboration: Using a
combination of questions and world knowledge to brainstorm logical solutions
to a task. f.
Sel-evaluative elaboration:
Judging self in relation to materials. g.
Creative elaboration: making up
a story line, or adopting a clever perspective. h.
Imagery: Using mental or actual
pictures or visuals to represent information; coded as a separate category,
but viewed as a form of elaboration. 8. Summarizing: Making a mental, oral, or written summary of language and
information presented in a task. 9. Translation: Rendering ideas from one language to another in a relatively
verbatim manner. 10. Transfer: Using previously acquired linguistic knowledge to facilitate a
language task. 11. Inferencing: Using available information to guess the meanings or usage of
unfamiliar language items associated with a language task, to predict
outcomes, or to fill in missing information. |
C. SOCIAL AND AFFECTIVE STRATEGIES: They involve interacting with another person to
assist learning or using affective control to assist a learning task. |
1. Questioning for clarification: Asking for explanation, verification, rephrasing,
or examples about the material; asking for clarification or verification
about the task; posing questions to the self. 2. Cooperation: Working together with peers to solve a problem, pool information,
check a learning task, model a language activity, or get feedback on oral or
written performance. 3. Self-talk: Reducing anxiety by using mental techniques that make one feel
competent to do a learning task. 4. Self-reinforcement: Providing personal motivation by arranging rewards for oneself when
a language learning activity has been successfully completed. |
O’Malley
and Chamot research work has also provided some very important conclusions:
-
Foreign
language (FL) students and ESL students showed similar patterns of
metacognitive and cognitive strategy use.
-
Students at all
levels reported using far more cognitive strategies than metacognitive ones.
-
In
metacognitive strategy use FL students predominantly reported using planning
strategies, such as selective attention, organizational planning, and
self-management.
-
In cognitive
strategy use, students at the beginning level of language study relied most on
repetition, translation and transfer, whereas more advanced students relied
most on inferencing.
-
Use of social
and affective strategies was reported much less frequently than use of
metacognitive strategies.
-
FL students of
all ability levels were found to use learning strategies. More effective
students used learning strategies more often and had a wider repertoire of
learning strategies than did less effective students.
-
Strategies
which involve simple operations on linguistic material, such as repetition and
memorization, or the use of formulaic language, seem to be the first acquired
and are the most frequently used in secondary school classrooms (O'Malley et
al. 1985a).
-
More
sophisticated strategies such as elaboration, monitoring, or grouping, etc.
which involve the transformation on material emerge later and are employed less
frequently.
-
Perhaps the
simpler strategies are available to all and training can influence their
frequency and appropriateness of use.
-
The more
complex strategies might not be available to everyone, and their use may have
to be explicitly taught to some students.
-
The use of
metacognitive strategies, the most exciting development in recent strategy
research, may not translate directly into easy application, since such
strategies may be the most demanding of all to teach to students and implement
effectively.
-
The use of
strategies reported by FL students is highly related to the type of instruction
they received in classrooms as cognitive strategies are directly related to
specific learning tasks. For example, in a classroom in which grammar is
emphasized, successful students would use deduction as a strategy in
applying rules to formulate correct sentences, and in a classroom in which
vocabulary acquisition or reading for details is emphasized, students would
find translation as a strategy to be effective. That brings into
prominence the role of teachers and the type of instruction students are
involved in. One consequence to be drawn from that is the importance of the
role played by teachers.
-
O'Malley et al.
report from their training study that it seems that strategy training can have
a direct influence on performance. Possibly the most trainable strategies are
those which have the quickest return and the less trainable strategies are
those associated with aspects of language learning and cognitive processes, and
whose effects can only be observed if they are used over extended periods of
time.
Helping students to become more
efficient when watching videos in English and develop as many strategies
(cognitive, metacognitive, social and affective) as possible (from
Madrid, D. and McLaren, N. (1997):
Making Progress 2, (p. 54). Valladolid: Ed. La Calesa.):
6.3.4. Oxford’s contribution (1990)
Oxford’s
(1990) work on strategies is perhaps the
most widely known among language teachers in recent years. In her book, she
presents a strategy system which is divided into two major classes: direct and
indirect. These two classes are subdivided into a total of six groups:
Direct class: -
memory strategies -
cognitive strategies -
compensation strategies Indirect class: -
Social strategies -
Affective strategies -
Metacognitive strategies |
According
to Oxford, direct and indirect strategies support each other and each strategy
group is capable of connecting with and
assisting every other strategy group, as follows (1990:15): Memory Strategies ,
Cognitive Strategies, Social Strategies, Compensation Strategies , Metacognitive Strategies, Affective
Strategies .
As
Oxford (1990) has noticed, at this stage in the short history of language
learning strategy research, there is not complete agreement on exactly what
strategies are, how many strategies exist; how they should be defined,
demarcated, and categorized; and whether it is –or ever will be- possible to
create a real, scientifically validated hierarchy of strategies. Even though
classification conflicts are inevitable, she proposes the following taxonomy
(1990:18-21):
DIRECT
STRATEGIES (Oxford, 1990)
(Memory, cognitive, compensation strategies) |
Group I: MEMORY
STRATEGIES
A.
Creating mental linkages: 1.
Grouping 2.
Associating/elaborating 3.
Placing new words into a
context B.
Applying images and sounds: 1.
Using imagery 2.
Semantic mapping 3.
Using keywords 4. Representing sounds in memory C.
Reviewing well: 1.
Structured reviewing D.
Employing action: 1.
Using physical response or
sensation 2. Using mechanical techniques |
Group II:
COGNITIVE STRATEGIES A.
Practicing: 1.
Repeating 2.
Formally practicing with sounds
and writing systems 3.
Recognizing and using formulas
and patterns 4.
Recombining 5.
Practicing naturalistically B. Receiving and sending
messages:
1.
Getting the idea quickly 2.
Using resources for receiving
and sending messages C. Analyzing and reasoning:
1.
Reasoning deductively 2.
Analysing expressions 3.
Analysing contrastively (across
languages) 4.
Translating 5.
Transfering D. Creating structure for input
and output:
1.
Taking notes 2.
Summarizing 3.
Highlighting |
Group III: COMPENSATION STRATEGIES A.
Guessing Intelligently: 1.
Using linguistic clues 2.
Using other clues B.
Overcoming limitations in
speaking and writing: 1.
Switching to the mother tongue 2.
Getting help 3.
Using mime or gesture 4.
Avoiding communication
partially or totally 5.
Selecting the topic 6.
Adjusting or approximating the
message 7.
Coining words 8.
Using a circumlocution or
synonym |
INDIRECT
STRATEGIES (Oxford, 1990)
(Metacognitive, Affective and Social Strategies) |
Group I:
METACOGNITIVE STRATEGIES
A.
Centering your learning: 1. Overviewing
and linking with already know material 2. Paying
attention 3. Delaying
speech production to focus on listening B. Arranging
and planning your learning: 1. Finding
out about language learning 2. Organizing 3. Setting
goals and objectives 4. Identifying
the purpose of a language task 5. Planning
for a language task 6. Seeking
practice opportunities C:
Evaluating your learning: 1. Self-monitoring 2. Self-evaluating |
Group II:
AFFECTIVE STRATEGIES A.
Lowering your anxiety: 1.
Using progressive relaxation,
deep breathing or meditation 2.
Using music 3.
Using laughter B.
Encouraging yourself: 1.
Making positive statements 2.
Taking risks wisely 3.
Rewarding yourself C.
Taking your emotional
temperature: 1.
Listening to your body 2.
Using a checklist 3.
Writing a language learning
diary 4.
Discussing your feelings with
someone else |
Group
III: SOCIAL STRATEGIES
A.
Asking questions: 1.
Asking for clarification 2.
Asking for correction B.
Cooperating with others: 1.
Cooperating with peers 2.
Cooperating with proficiency
users of the new language C.
Empathizing with others: 1.
Developing cultural
understanding 2.
Becoming aware of others
thoughts and feelings |
Read Oxford’s taxonomy again and select six
activities in an English textbook so that each activity or exercise implies the
development of a strategy belonging to these groups of strategies:
-
Memory
strategies -
Social strategies
-
Cognitive
strategies -
Affective strategies
-
Compensation
strategies -
Metacogniitve strategies
___________________________________________________________________________
According
to Valcárcel et al. (1996), Oxford's
work is not very clear as regards her treatment of cognitive strategies since
the classification scheme she suggests continually confuses the concepts of
processes, strategies and techniques, from the point of view we outlined above.
What it would seem to be is more of a good summary of all the possible strategies
and techniques so far identified by other researchers, with examples of each
strategy linking them with each of the four language skills, than a systematic
approach to learning strategies with empirically valid research results to
justify this particular classification. However, having said that, the block of
strategies she refers to as 'indirect strategies' including metacognitive,
affective and social strategies does seem to be a more clarifying, systematic
and relevant grouping.
6.3.5. Valcárcel,
Coyle and Verdú’s taxonomy (1996)
For
Valcárcel et al. (1996), the cognitive component of procedural knowledge
comprising LA depends on mental processes involved in hypothesis formation
through reception of new L2 knowledge (internalizing input), hypothesis
formation through production (interaction) and automatization through
language use (output). In the following tables they offer a tentative
proposal of a possible classification of those processes, strategies and
techniques taken from the currently available taxonomies, trying to link
the ones closely related with the instructional process (see Gagné 1974 and
Beltrán 1993: 42) with those identified in the literature on SLA and which they
consider as most applicable to the field of classroom foreign language
learning.
PROCESSES |
STRATEGIES |
TECHNIQUES |
|||||
I. SENSITIVIZATION (Motivation) |
|||||||
1. PLANNING |
Planning learning tasks Organizational planning Self-management Self-monitoring Problem identification |
- Setting goals and
objectives. - Identifiying the purpose of a task. - Planning the parts, sequence, main ideas, to be
used in handling task. - Identifiying and controlling one's language performance. - Evaluate one's own capacity. - Checking, verifying or correcting one's errors. - Identifying the central points needing resolution
in a task. |
|||||
2. AFFECTIVE |
Emotional control Encouraging oneself Self-responsibility Self-reinforcement Decision taking |
- Self-talk to lower one's learning anxiety. - Making positive statements. - Taking risks wisely. - Rewarding oneself. - Writing language learning diary. - Discussing learning feelings with someone else. - Active participation in learning tasks. - Self-evaluation - Giving priorities to learning needs |
|||||
3. SOCIAL |
Questioning for clarification Appeal for assistance Cooperation Empathizing with others |
- Asking for explanation, or verification. - Asking for correction - Working together with peers to solve a problem,
pool information, model a language activity or get feedback on oral or written performance. - Becoming aware of others' thoughts and feelings. - Developing cultural understanding. |
|||||
II. ACQUISITION (Codification) |
|||||||
1. COMPREHENSION |
Directed attention Intensive attention Selective attention Guessing |
- Exploring / setting aims of learning task. - Getting global information. - Activation of previous knowledge. - Physical response. - Underlining, ticking. - Taking notes. - Matching. - Making associations: using clues, using imagery. |
|||||
2. RETENTION |
Memorization Imitation |
- Grouping, semantic mapping. - Key word method. - Placing new words into context. - Reviewing. - Speaking to self. - Repeating a language model overt practice - Silent rehearsal. - Using formulas & patterns. |
|||||
3. CONSTRUCTION |
Elaboration Interaction Analysis Resourcing |
- Recombining. - Substitution. - Note-taking. - Paraphrasing. - Display & referential questions. - Practising patterns in pairs and groups. - Analysing contrastively L1>< L2. - Inducing meaning and rules from context. - Using dictionaries, grammar indexes, textbooks. |
|||||
4. TRANSFORMING |
Translating Inferencing Extending Summarizing |
- Rendering ideas from one language to another. - Applying rules & meaning deductively. - Deducing meaning from existing knowledge. - Building texts (oral/written) with help of cues. - Making reports from received information. |
|||||
III. AUTOMATIZATION |
|||||||
1. TRANSFER |
Low level transfer High level Transfer |
- Apply knowledge to seemingly similar tasks
(semicontrolled production). - Apply knowledge to different tasks (free production). |
|||||
2. LANGUAGE USE FOR COMMUNICATION |
Overcoming limitations in speaking and writing |
- Guessing from context. - Getting help. - Code-switching. - Foreignizing words. - Using mime or gesture. - Coding words. - Planning discourse in advance. - Restructuring discourse. - Using a circumlocution or synonym. - Simplifying or avoiding L2 rules. |
|||||
IV. EVALUATION
|
|||||||
1. SELF-ASSESMENT |
Of products Of processes Initial Formative Summative |
- Checking the outcomes of one's languague
performance - Checking strategy use or ability to perform the
task at hand. - Diagnosis and detection of abilities and
requirements. - Interaction evaluation-learning - Evaluation of goal achievement. |
|||||
The role assigned by some authors to LS seems to be
fundamental. For example, Brown et al. (1983) conclude that the strategies, or
the deliberate plans and routines used in learning, remembering, and problem
solving are the primary determinants of learning outcomes. They also highlight
the distinction between cognitive and metacognitive strategies.
As we have illustrated in the previous taxonomies, the former, in O’Malley and
Chamot’s words, “involve interacting
with the material to be learned, manipulating the material mentally or
physically, or applying a specific technique to a learning task”, the latter,
“involve thinking about the learning process, planning for learning, monitoring
the learning task, and evaluating how well one has learned” (see O’Malley and
Chamot’s taxonomy).
Consequently, metacognitive
strategies involve both the knowledge about learning (metacognitive
knowledge) and control or evaluation over learning (metacognitive strategies). Metacognitive
knowledge refers to knowledge of one’s own cognitive processes. According
to Brown et al. (1983) it is stable, thus it is retrievable for use with
learning tasks. It is also statable, that is it can be reflected upon
and used as the topic of discussion with others. Nevertherless, it may be fallible,
so that what one believes about one’s cognitive processes may be inaccurate,
such as the belief that simple rote repetition is the key that underlies all
learning. It seems to appear late in development, since the ability of learners
to step back from learning and reflect on their cognitive processess may
require prior learning experiences as a point of reference.
But regulation of learning, as
distinguished from knowledge about learning, entails the use of metacognitive
strategies. These include in O’Malley and Chamot’s taxonomy the following:
1. Planning 2. Directed attention 3.
Selective attention 4.
Self-management 5.
Self-monitoring 6.
Problem identification 7. Self-evaluation |
According to
O’Malley and Chamot, metacognitive strategies do not necessarily share the
qualities of being stable and statable with metacognitive
knowledge, and may be more task- and age-dependent. In the following section we
provide techniques to help the students develop their metacognition in
connection to the learning activities and learning strategies that they are
constantly using to learn English.
TASK 13
-
How
efficient are you as an English learner? How many strategies do you usually use
for learning? Read the Valcárcel et al. taxonomy, the column headed with
STRATEGIES in a vertical direction, and grade yourself by writing the
correspondent score by each strategy. Use the following scale:
5 = always 4 = very often 3 = usually 2 = seldom 1 = never
When you have
graded yourself according to the 36 strategies, sum up the scores and get the
total. Was your total low or high? (notice that the possible maximum score is
180).
-
Now
work out the subtotals for each of the three groups and each of the ten
processes. Which of the ten PROCESSES obtained
the lowest score? So, what techniques should you use to improve your
learning?
________________________________________________________________________________
Anderson,
C. W. (1987): “Strategic teaching in
science”. In B. F. Jones, A. S. Palincsar, D. S. Ogle, and E. G. Carr (Eds.), Strategic
teaching and learning: cognitive instruction in the content areas (pp.
73-91). Alexandria, Va.: Association for Supervision and Curriculum
Development.
Anderson,
J. R. (1976): Language, memory, and
thought. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.
Anderson,
J. R. (1978): “Arguments concerning
representations for mental imagery”. Psychological Review, 85:249-277.
Anderson,
J. R. (1980): Cognitive psychology
and its implications. San Francisco: Freeman.
Anderson,
J. R. (1982): “Acquisition of
cognitive skills”. Psychological Review 89:369-406.
ANDERSON, J. R. (1983): The Architecture of Cognition. Cambridge,
Mass.: Harvard University Press.
Anderson,
J. R. (1983): The architecture of
cognition. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.
Anderson,
J. R. (1985): Cognitive psychology and its
implications. 2nd ed. New York: Freeman.
Anderson, J. R. (Ed.) (1981). Cognitive skills and their acquisition. Hillsdale,
N.J.: Erlbaum.
BELTRÁN, J.A. (1993): Procesos, estrategias y técnicas de aprendizaje.
Madrid. Editorial Síntesis.
BIALYSTOK, E. (1981): The role of conscious strategies in second
language proficency. Modern Language Journal 65:24-35.
BIALYSTOK, E. (1990): Communication Strategies. Oxford: Basil Blackwell
BROWN, A., J. D. BRANSFORD, R. A. FERRARA and J. C. CAMPIONE (1983): 'Learning,
remembering, and understanding'. In J.H. Flavell and M. Markman (eds), Carmichaels
manual of child psychology. Vol. 3, 77-166. New York: Wiley.
CHESTERFIELD R. and K. B. CHESTERFIELD (1985): 'Natural order in childrens' use of second
language learning strategies'. Applied Linguistics, 6, 1: 12-26.
DANSEREAU, D.F. (1985):
'Learning strategies Research'. In J.W. Segal et al. Thinking and Learning
skills. Vol I Hillsdale. Erlbaum,
ELLIS, R. (1985): Understanding Second Language Acquisition.
New York: Oxford University Press.
ELLIS, R. (1994): The Study of Second Language Acquisition.
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
FAERCH, C. and G. KASPER (1980):
'Processes in foreign language learning and communication'. Interlanguage
Studies Bulletin 5: 47-118.
FAERCH, C. and G. KASPER (1984): Learner
Language and Language Learning. Multilingual Matters. Clevedon.
FAERCH, C. and G. KASPER (1985):
'Procedural knowledge as a component of foreign language learners'
communicative competence'. In H.Boete and W.Herrlitz (eds). Kommunikation im
(Sprach-) Unterricht. Utrecht.
FAERCH, C. and G. KASPER (1986): 'The
role of comprehension in second language learning' Applied Linguistics 7/3:257-274
FAERCH, C. and G. KASPER (1987): 'From product to process -introspective
methods in second language research'. In C. Faerch and G. Kasper (eds.)
Introspection in Second language Research. Philadelphia, Pa:
Multilingual Matters.
FAERCH, C. and G. KASPER (eds.)(1983): Strategies
in Interlanguage Communication. Harlow, Essex: Longman
GAGNÉ, E.D. (1974): Essentials of
learning instruction. New York: Dryden Press.
GAGNÉ, E.D. (1985): The Cognitive
Psychology of School Learning. Boston. Mass.: Little Brown.
JIMENEZ, M.
(1993): El diario del aprendiz como instrumento de entrenamiento cognitivo
en la adquisición del ingles. Ph. Diss. University of Granada: Departamento
de Filología Inglesa.
KRASHEN, S. D. (1982): Principles and
Practice in Second Language Acquisition. Oxford: Pergamon.
KRASHEN, S. D. (1985): The Input Hypothesis:
Issues and Implications. Harlow, Essex: Longman.
KRASHEN, S.D. (1981): Second Language
Acquisition and Second Language Learning. Oxford: Pergamon.
Lachman, R., Lachman, J. L., and Butterfield, E.
C. (1979): Cognitive psychology and information
processing. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.
MADRID, D., PÉREZ, C., MUROS, J. and CORDOVILA, A. (1998): Education through English. Granada: Grupo Editorial Universitario.
MANCHÓN RUIZ, R. M. and BRUTON, A. (eds.) (1993): Serie sobre estrategias de aprendizaje y
uso del lenguaje. Sevilla, Alcalá de Guadaira: CEP
MARTINEZ, A. and M. VALCÁRCEL (1992, 1993): 'Propuesta de Secuencia A' in Propuesta de
Secuencia de Lenguas Extranjeras: Primaria y Secundaria obligatoria.
Ministerio de Educación y Ciencia. Editorial Escuela Española: 9-125; 9-174.
McLAREN, N. And MADRID, D.
(1996): A Handbook for TEFL. Alicante,
Alcoy: Marfil.
NAIMAN, N., M FRÖHLICH, H. STERN and A. TODESCO (1978): The Good Language Learner. Toronto:
Ontario Institute for Studies in Education.
O'MALLEY J. M., A. U. CHAMOT and L. KUPPER (1989): 'Listening
comprehension strategies in second language acquisition'. Applied Linguistics 110/4:418-437
O'MALLEY, J. M. and A. U. CHAMOT (1990): Learning Strategies in Second Language Acquisition.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
O'MALLEY, J. M., A. U. CHAMOT, G. STEWNER-MANZANARES, L. KUPPER and R.
P. RUSSO (1985a): 'Learning strategies
used by beginning and intermediate ESL students'. Language Learning, 35,
1, 21-46.
OXFORD, R. (1990): Language Learning
Strategies. Newbury House. New York.
RUBIN, J. (1975): 'What the 'Good Language
Learner' can teach us', TESOL Quarterly, 9, 1, 41-51.
RUBIN, J. (1981): 'Study of cognitive processes in second language
learning', Applied Linguistics, 11: 117-31.
RUBIN, J. and THOMPSON, I. (1982): Ho to be a more successful
language learner. Boston, Mass.: Heinle & Heinle.
Shuell, T. J. (1986): Cognitive conceptions of learning. Review of Educational
Research 56:411-36.
SKEHAN, P. (1989): Individual differences in Second-Language
Learning. Edward Arnold.
STERN, H.H. (1983): Fundamental Concepts
of Language Teaching. Oxford: O.U.P.
STERN, H.H. (1992): Issues and Options in Language Teaching.
Oxford: O.U.P.
TARONE, E. (1981): Some thoughts on the
notion of communication strategy. TESOL Quarterly 15:285-295.
TARONE, E. (1988): Variation in Interlanguage. London: Arnold.
VALCÁRCEL, M., COYLE, Y. AND VERDÚ, M. (1996): “Learning Foreign Languages:
Learner Strategies”, ein McLaren, N and Madrid, D.
Weinstein, C. E., and Mayer, R. E. (1986): The teaching of learning strategies. In M. C.
Wittrock (Ed.), Handbook of research on teaching (pp. 315-27). 3rd ed.
New York: Macmillan.
WENDEN, A. (1991): Learner Strategies for
Learner Autonomy. New York: Prentice Hall.
WENDEN, A. and J. RUBIN (eds.) (1987):
Learner Strategies in Language Learning. Englewood Cliffs, N. J.:
Prentice Hall.
WONG-FILLMORE, L. (1976): The Second Time
Around: Cognitive and social Strategies in Second Language Acquisition,
Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Standford University.
WONG-FILLMORE, L. (1979): 'Individual differences
in second language acquisition'. In Fillmore C.J., Wang W-S.Y., and Kempler D.
(eds) Individual Differences in Language Ability and Language Behaviour,
New York: Academic Press.