Madrid, D. (2000): “Observation and research in the classroom”, en Teaching English as a Foreign Language, Barcelona: The Australian Institute. pp. 1-100.

 

 

---

INTRODUCCIÓN A LA INVESTIGACIÓN EN EL AULA DE LENGUA EXTRANJERA

Daniel Madrid

(Universidad de Granada)

 

 

INTRODUCCIÓN

 

In this unit we will be examining how and why teachers should be encouraged to reflect and analyse  their own teaching and the classroom context. We will look at how the notion of research in ELT has developed, and how a new paradigm of reflective, classroom- based research has evolved.

 

It is assumed  that teachers can improve their knowledge and skills, not necessarily by following a particular method or the findings provided by “official” research, but with an enquiring attitude which allows him/her to find out the most adequate techniques in each situation. Because each teaching situation is different and needs to take a range of factors into account, from individual students' characteristics to those of the teacher, the classroom context and the community with its local educational and intellectual traditions. And every language lesson is necessarily different and unique as it is jointly constructed by the teacher and the learners (Allwright 1991).

 

Nowadays, the theory of constructivism is not only applied to the learning processes, but also to the teaching situations. It is widely accepted  that each teacher constructs his/her own 'theory' about teaching and learning. These ideas comes from a variety of sources:

-         from the teacher's personal beliefs and value system,

-         from his/her experience as a learner,

-         from training or education,

-         and from his/her classroom practice and experience.

 

It is this latter area - that of classroom practice - that we will be primarily focussing on in this unit. In fact, Stern (1983) argues that thinking about classroom practice is an essential part of learning to theorise. Thus a reflective approach to our teaching is very much a product of the post-method condition of ELT (see Unit 5 for a discussion of the end of method).

 

2.2       WHAT IS RESEARCH?

 

All of us often question ourselves about lots of daily life events that we do not quite understand or seek answers to questions that other people make. In this sense, we all become researchers of our daily life when we try to find out explanations for the  phenomena happening around us. In doing this, we observe these events and analyse them. We make guesses and adventure hypothesis that we later check till we can be sure that what we initially assumed is true under certain conditions. So, the basic components of research in our daily life  can be summarised as follows (see Seliger and Shohamy, 1989:7)

 

 

1.        There is a phenomenon that is not clearly understood.

2.        To try to understand it, the phenomenon is observed and analysed.

3.        In this process we make questions about the phenomenon: Why, how, when, ... does it happen? What’s the connection with other events? Etc.

4.        Several guesses,  answers and possible explanations are anticipated intuitively.

5.        One (or some) of the anticipated explanations is/are considered that most probably explain(s) the phenomenon (hypothesis).

6.        We check the hypothesis in other situations: collect more data and test to what extent the hypothesis can be maintained.

 

 

 

Even though we have seen a close connection between scientific research and research in every day life, there are important differences, as Seliger and Shohamy have pointed out (1989:10):

 

The differences between knowledge arrived at through common sense and intuition on the one hand, and scientific research on the other, can be expressed by concepts such as “organised”, “structured”, “methodical”, “systematic”, “testable” and specifically by the notion of disciplinary inquiry.

 

It is also important to differentiate the type of research we decide to  do:

 

a)      Basic or theoretical research: it aims to construct abstract theoretical models which explain second language teaching and learning.

b)      Applied research: it applies the theoretical models provided by  a) basic research to different   fields of study (e.g. education).

c)      Practical research: it makes a practical utilization of a) theoretical research and b) applied research. For example, by testing classroom hypothesis,  controlling variables, providing pedagogical principles experimented in the classroom, etc. It must be noticed that practical research  is based on the premises established by theoretical and applied  research and it is influenced by them when it is developed in classroom situations.

 

Classroom research is normally practical research which is centred on the classroom. It aims to explains what actually happens inside the classroom,  the direct and indirect influence  of internal and external factors related to the student, the teacher and the ELT curriculum. As van Lier points out (1988) we know very little about what  is going on in classrooms, so classroom research becomes an important tool  to explain the relation between the diversity of variables that continuously interact:

 

As yet we know too little about all the variables that play a role in all the classrooms to be able to make rash recommendations about methods of teaching and ways of learning (1988:7)

 

On the other hand, classroom research has also been considered one of the most difficult places to do research, hence its consideration as a “black box” as far as the connection between the input received by the students and the output they produce. In addition to that, and as we will see in the section about research methods, the concept of research itself  has not been unproblematic. Richterich, a Swiss scholar closely involved in the Council of Europe Modern Languages Project (see Unit 4), and himself involved in research, pointed out some of the piffalls involved:

 

Apart from these disadvantages, there was a growing awareness among both researchers and teachers of the inadequacy of a scientific, rationalist model of research, which we will examine later.

 

 

2.3       THE SCOPE OF CLASSROOM RESEARCH

 

When we aim to study what happens in our English class we must take into consideration a great variety of variables that may have a direct or indirect influence or relationship between one another.  According to van Lier (1988:27) some of the central topics to research about are the following:

a)      The nature and development of  the students’ interlanguage.

b)      The role of interaction between the teacher and the students, that is, the type of communication which takes place in the classroom

c)      The use of learning strategies

d)      Cognitive styles

e)      Efficiency of evaluation techniques

 

We think that a more complete picture of the scope of classroom research is illustrated in  figure 1. Here, we present a model which includes the main groups of variables that must be controlled in some way:  by  qualitative, ethnographic and naturalistic procedures or  experimental and  quantitative ones (Madrid, 1995:60-62; 1998a, 1998b).

 

Fig. 1: A framework for L2 teaching analysis and research

 

PRESAGE AND CONTEXT VARIABLES

     

2. THE TEACHER

 

-   Individual characteristics

-   Beliefs, preparation, personality

 

1. THE LEARNER

 

- Social context

- Individual characteristics: cognitive styles, motivation, personality

 
             

     

                                                          

                                 

                                 

 

 


                                                            PROCESS VARIABLES                     

                                                                                                  SLL     

                          SLA

3. LEARNING PROCESS

ACQUISITION PROCESS

 

-   Mental processes

-  Learning strategies

 

4. THE CLASSROOM

 

-   Educational treatment

-   L2 curriculum

-   Tasks

-   Materials

 

 
 


                                                            

 

 

 

 

 

 


                                                            PRODUCT VARIABLES        

 

5. RESULTS / ATTAINMENT / COMPETENCE / PROFICIENCY

 

-   Principles, concepts:

grammar, functions, cultural aspects, ...

-  Procedures, skills:

listening, speaking, reading and writing

-  Attitudes and values

 
 

 

 

 

 

 


       

   

The model presented in this diagram distinguishes five sets of variables that interact. At the top, are the learner and the teacher, the two main protagonists in the teaching and learning processes.

 

If the learner acquires the L2 in a naturalistic setting, through communication in natural social situations, without the teacher's guide and outside the classroom setting, then he/she develops learning strategies and mental processes that lead to what is called second language acquisition (SLA) (Krashen 1981). This sequence only involves the factors included in boxes 1, 4 & 5: the learner, his/her learning process and his/her results.

 

An alternative sequence takes place when the learner becomes a student and is guided by a teacher in a classroom formal setting. In this case the L2 learning takes place through conscious study, with the help of the L2 teacher and some teaching resources: books, recordings, etc. This educational treatment is likely to develop different strategies and learning processes, and consequently, different outcomes. This sequence, illustrated in boxes 1, 2, 3, 4 & 5 is said to produce second (or foreign) language learning (SLL) (Krashen 1981).

 

Though, in many situations, it is difficult (or impossible) to know if SLA or SLL takes place, this distinction is useful, especially when we want to differentiate the type of context or setting in which L2 learning takes place.

 

The model presented in the diagram was first proposed by Dunkin and Biddle and adapted later by Stern (1983:338 and 500). Dunkin and Biddle used the following four terms for the set of variables that we have identified in fig. 1(1975:38):

 

 

 a) Presage variables

‑ Teacher formative experiences and training

‑ Teacher properties: skills, personality

 

 b) Context variables

‑ Pupil formative experiences

‑ Pupil properties: abilities, attitudes, etc.

‑ School context

 

 c) Process variables

‑ Teacher classroom behaviour (didactic performance)

‑ Pupil classroom behaviour

‑ Learning processes

 

 d) Product variables

‑ Outcomes/final competence/proficiency

 

 

2.4       METHODS AND PARADIGMS FOR CLASSROOM RESEARCH

 

As we will see, research questions and topics can be looked into from many different perspectives, by following different approaches and by using different procedures. The approach that we adopt needs to be suitable for the kind of research we want to carry out, for the variables we want to control. In some occasions, an observational process will be enough because the data we want to collect cannot be quantified, but in others we may need to illustrate our findings with figures and a statistic treatment may be necessary. So, the approach we adopt depends on the nature of the research we aim to do. Very often an eclectic position or a combination of paradigms may be necessary.  As classroom research is defined primarily by its setting, the classroom provides the focal point for the types of data collected. However, a wide variety of approaches are used to obtain and analyse the data, and the choice of approach depends upon many factors: the researcher's philosophy, the issue to be investigated, the constraints inherent in the situation and so on.

 

First ELT research studies

 

The beginnings of a research approach in language teaching can be dated back to the late 19 century, linked to the development of the language sciences and the scientific movement in education. However, a truly consistent and deliberate research approach in ELT only became apparent from the 1950s, and it is this that we will now look at.

 

One of the first attempts at systematic research was carried out in 1948 by Agard and Dunkel at the University of Chicago, in which 'new' and traditional methods of language teaching were compared. As a result of this study, Dunkel put together a volume of all the studies on language learning to date (1948). In the same year a journal with a strong research orientation, "Language Learning", was published by the English Language Institute at the University of Michigan in Ann Arbor. Its director, Charles Fries, had been a key figure during the previous decade in giving language pedagogy a basis in research (see Unit 2).

 

Nevertheless, despite these early attempts, research into the field of ELT remained thin until the sixties, when a radical change took place, and research began to influence policy issues and the method debate in second language education. This was due to the fact that during the fifties and sixties language centres with a strong research orientation had been set up, such as the CREDIF (Centre de Recherche et d'Etude pour Ia Diffusion du Français) in France in 1951, or the CAL (Center for Applied Linguistics) in Washington, D.C. in 1959, or the Centre for Information on Language Teaching and Research in 1966, pIus a number of university centres in applied linguistics, in Britain.

 

Thus when the Audiolingual method (see Unit 2) appeared on the scene in the late 1950s, considerable interest was aroused, and a number of major investigations were carried out in an attempt to resolve controversies about its merits over the more traditional grammar translation method.

 

Two other areas of language teaching which began to be systematically researched from the 1960s on were those of language teaching for younger children, and the effectiveness of immersion teaching programmes.

 

The problem of approach: Objectivity and subjectivity; quantitative versus qualitative research

 

The problem of approach can be addressed in terms of potentially opposing viewpoints on how research should be conducted. The difference between these perspectives hinge primarily on differing attitudes towards intervention and control. The issues of subjectivity and objectivity often arise when devising observation schedules for recording classroom data. In constructing such instruments, researchers distinguish between "low inference" and "high inference" categories of analysis (Long, 1980a). Low inference categories include things that can be counted or coded without the observer having to infer very much (the number of times a certain student raises his or her hand, for example). "High inference categories, on the other hand (learner's attention, for example), demand that the observer make a judgement that goes well beyond what is immediately visible. Generally speaking, in developing a coding procedure, it makes sense to use low inference categories wherever possible, and try to document all the evidence that leads us to make broader inferences. A universal problem for classroom researchers, then, is one of finding low inference means of investigating non-trivial aspects of what happens in language classes.

 

The terms quantitative  and qualitative apply to both the data collection and data analysis phases of an investigation. Any sort of measurement that yields numerical information generates quantitative data. On the other hand, some data are not the product of measurement or counting and thus do not result in numerical information (prose descriptions, diaries, and so on). This kind of data are qualitative.

 

Likewise, once we have collected data we can analyse them by counting or measuring (quantitative analysis), or by directly reflecting upon and trying to interpret them (a qualitative analysis). Various combinations of quantitative and qualitative data collection and analysis are possible.

 

Qualitative data, such as a set of diaries kept by learners, would typically be considered subjective, a record of opinions and perceptions, rather than "facts". For some researchers learners' diaries are of interest not because they hold the "truth" about something, but precisely because they are a record of opinions and perceptions important to the learners -ideas which cannot easily be tapped in other ways.

 

Quantification has a similar appeal to objectivity in that if you can count things then you can subject them to rigorous statistical analysis using procedures set out in statistical manuals. If you get the procedures right, people can challenge you on your interpretation but the figures themselves cannot be denied. The problem, here, of course, is that not everything can be counted or measured adequately, and therefore numbers cannot tell the whole story. Some vital element may be missing. Also, there is the difficult problem that statistical procedures are themselves the subject of endless controversy. Even deciding on a unit of analysis for counting can be very tricky. Experimental research is objective and quantitative, whereas action research tends to use subjective, qualitative and ethnographic techniques.

 

Eclectic approaches to research

 

Of course very few studies can be confidently characterized as being purely experimental (+ structured, - controlled) or purely naturalistic (- structured, -controlled). In fact, in recent years observational procedures from naturalistic enquiry have been used to document the process variables involved in implementing the treatment in product-oriented experimental research.

While there are as yet relatively few examples of this combined approach, the report by Bailey et al. (1990) demonstrates that it is becoming increasingly clear in general educational research (though research on language teaching and learning has lagged behind somewhat in this area) that action research is often a viable alternative, and one which offers immediate rewards to teachers and learners.

From all that we have said on the topic, it should be clear that we see most value in investigations that combine objective and subjective elements, that quantify only what can be usefully quantified, and that utilise qualitative data collection and analysis procedures wherever they are appropriate. This eclectic approach has been emphasied in several occasions:

 

“Ideally experimental and interpretative research should be convergent rather than parallel or divergent lines of enquiry” (va Lier, 1988/90:XIV).

 

“There is no need to oppose qualitative and quantitative research. Each is capable of “critical thinking” and each has its place in IL (interlanguage) studies. The danger is ... in failing to acknowledge the contribution that can be made by “hybrid” research (i.e. research that employs both qualitative and quantitative procedures) (Ellis, 1984:284).

 

“It should be clear that we see most value in investigations that combine objective and subjective elements, that quantify only what can be usefully quantified, and that utilise qualitative date collection and analysis procedures wherever they are appropriate” (Allwright y Baley, 1991:67).

 

 

2.4.1 EXPERIMENTAL RESEARCH

 

Here the researcher exerts a high degree of control and purposefully intervenes in the setting, to determine the effect on intervention. It is important to note, however, that "intervention" is by no means a negative thing. It is simply a technical terms which refers to the "treatment" administered to some subjects (the "experimental group") in order to test a hypothesis about a cause-and effect relationship. This treatment is withheld from other subjects in the study (typically called -the control group"). If the researchers are careful in setting up the study, these two groups can be presumed to be virtually identical in all respects, except that one gets the treatment (teaching method, materials, for example) and the other does not. After the treatment has been implemented, a test of some sort is usually administered to both groups and their results are compared. Then various mathematical procedures are used to determine whether or not there are statistically significant differences in the test scores of the two groups. From the results, the paradigm claims, we can infer that the treatment either did or did not cause a measurable change in behaviour or learning (the hypothesis effect).

 

 In experimental research, there are some preparatory stages or phases that are currently followed. These stages include the following steps (Seliger and Shohamy, 1989):

 

 

1)       Formulating the general question or the problem which will be solved. This usually emerges from the researcher’s experience, from research paradigms or from sources outside the second language field.

2)       Feasibility of the research work we aim to carry out to solve the problem.

3)       Deciding on the objectives that will be achieved.

4)      Formulating the research plan and hypothesis to be tested.

 

 

 

In addition to that, research has to be contextualized. According to Seliger and Shohamy it means (1989:85):

-         selecting a research problem, 

-         to broaden the perspective of the research,

-         a revision of the existing literature on the topic,

-         to create a rationale for the study,

-         to help the researcher narrow down the research question in preparation for conducting the research.

-         to describe the different sources for locating the literature:

-         references to existing material, such as indices, computer searches, and bibliographies,

-         and the actual material such as journal articles and reviews.

-         to describe the criteria for determining the relevance of the material to the research topic,

-         and suggestions on ways of organizing and reporting the literature review .

 

Another key stage of research begins after the research question or hypothesis has been developed.  Planning the research takes place after the researcher has identified the focus or objective of the research. In synthetic or analytic-deductive research, planning requires the careful development of a plan in which those factors to be controlled or manipulated are identified:  the independent, dependent, subject, and extraneous variables. 

The dependent variable is the means by which any changes are measured. The independent variable is the factor the  researcher manipulates in order to see what effect the changes introduced will have. For example, if we want to study the relationship between the students participation and their degree of extroversion  in their  final results, we may start the research plan  by thinking that classroom participation and extroversion encourage good results in SLL (hypothesis). The degree of participation and extroversion are independent variables and the student’s achievement is the dependent variable.

 

Research which focuses on variables, makes predictions and tests hypothesis is primarily of the deductive type. Heuristic research approaches the research context from a different perspective, without preconceptions, with the aim of generating hypothesis but not to test them, using qualitative  methods. It  does not attempt to control or manipulate variables (see Seliger and Shohamy 1989).

 

The following checklist can be very useful for research analysis (Seliger and Shohamy 1989: 80-81):

 

A About the research topic

1        What is the main research area?

2        What is the research problem?

3        What are the major research questions or hypotheses?

 

B About the research context

1        What other research studies were conducted in the same area?

2        What were their main findings?

3        What is the rationale of the research?

4        Why was it important to conduct the research?

 

C About the research method

1        What are the main variables of the study?

2       Which research design was used? (experimental, correlational, des­criptive, multivariate, ethnographic?)

3        Description of the population, sample, and selection procedures

4       The data collection procedures - information about their development reliability, validity, pilot study

5        Description of the data collected

 

D About the data analysis

1        What are the specific data analysis procedures used?

2        Were they quantitative or qualitative, or both?

 

E About the findings

1        What were the main findings?

2        What does the researcher conclude from them?

3        How do the findings relate to the research context and to the underlying theories?

4        What are the implications of the findings?

5        What recommendations does the researcher make based on the findings?

6        What recommendations are drawn from the results?

 

F Criticism of the research

             Consideration of A-E above and specifically:

1        the statement of the problem

2        the identification of the hypotheses

3        the description and definition of the variables

4        the appropriacy of the design of the study

5        the appropriacy of the instruments

6        the appropriacy of the data analysis procedures

7        the consistency of the results with the analysis

8        whether the conclusion, implications, and recommendations are warranted by the results.

 

Questioning the scientific research paradigm

 

The success of modern science has had the effect of imbuing anything that is 'scientific' with the flavour of absolute truth. Thus research which is based on rational, 'scientific' experiment is seen as unquestionable, objective, right. Why should this be so? According to Winograd and Flores:

 

The rationalist orientation... is also regarded, perhaps because of the prestige and success that modern science enjoys, as the very paradigm of what it means to think and be intelligent... It is scarcely surprising, then, that the rationalistic orientation pervades not only artificial intelligence and the rest of computer science, but also much of llnguistics, management theory, and cognitive science... rationalistic styles of discourse and thinking have determined the questions that have been asked and the theories, methodologies, and assumptions that have been adopted. (1986:16)

 

However, 'scientific' research has so far failed to answer some of the most pressing questions in language teaching. The issue of discipline in the classroom is a case in point. The scientific model of research would try to use scientific knowledge to solve the problem of maintaining discipline: thus the theorists Stones and Morris, writing in 1972, claimed that "(this) important area of classroom and group management has received detailed empirical study, and a body of theoretical and practical information has been amassed which begins to put the problems of discipline on a scientific footing..." (1972:14). The implication here is that scientific study would soon provide a formula for dealing with classroom discipline problems. As all practising teachers know, this has not been the case. Thus scientific research seems to promise solutions to very complex professional dilemmas, but rarely manages to deliver the goods.

 

Another weakness that is apparent in a scientific research paradigm is that of its lack of grounding in specific classroom practice. There has traditionally been a separation between theorists who do research on the one hand, and practitioners - the teachers in the classroom - on the other. Researchers tend to be working in university departments where they have no contact with the daily realities of the language classroom, particularly as represented in primary and secondary schooling. Researchers and practitioners often have very different training, and there is frequently a sense of status attached to the job of researcher which is not to be found in the job of classroom teacher. In Schon's words:

 

It was to be the business of university based scientists and scholars to create the fundamental theory which professionals and technicians would apply to practice... But this division of labour reflected a hierarchy of kinds of knowledge which was also a ladder of status. (1983:36)

 

Thus there has been a tendency in research circles to downgrade the value of the classroom teacher's expertise which is derived from experience, rather than from research.

 

At the same time, research findings are in a sense imposed on teachers. Teachers are called upon to implement new ideas and theories in language teaching in the form of teaching materials, rather than asked to consider the ideas themselves and to evaluate them critically in the light of their own experience and teaching context.

 

Ellis (1990) points to two reasons for the growing scepticism with the conventional rational research paradigm: firstly, the relationship between teaching and learning is extremely complex. It is not linear (i.e. teaching does not automatically lead to learning), thus experimental research can only provide us with an understanding of fragments of the language learning process, not the whole process. Secondly, according to Ellis, the findings from formal experiments conducted under laboratory conditions are not necessarily applicable to the language classroom:

 

Innovation in the classroom can never be just a question of implementing a recommendation derived from research. It is always a process of negotiation, involving the teacher's overall educational ideology, the learner's expectations and preferences and local constraints  that determine what is feasible. There is no single pedagogical solution which is applicable to all classrooms. (Ellis 1990:68)

 

Classroom research of any kind is very likely to be a sensitive business, however carefully it is done, because being observed in any way is anxiety-provoking, to say the least, and being closely observed, recorded or analysed is enough to put anyone on the defensive. In any given classroom study, the researcher must decide if the learners are going to be treated like subjects in a classical psychology experiment and kept uninformed about what the experimenter is investigating, or -at the opposite end of the continuum, as in the action research paradigm- if they are going to be involved as full collaborators from the start, helping to define problems, deciding how to approach them, and so on.

 

 

2.4.2    CLASSROOM OBSERVATION

 

When the teaching and learning of a foreign language takes place in a formal setting, it is necessary to observe and analyse systematically what is going on in order to understand such teaching and learning processes.  Classroom observation has been used with different purposes: to compare teaching methods, to study the most efficient classroom techniques, to evaluate teachers and materials, etc. All these topics are fundamental components of classroom research. So, observation becomes one of the most important techniques to study what actually happens inside the classroom in a systematic way. In addition to direct observation, there are other procedures for classroom research. These include surveys and self-reports. Self-report data are obtained by conducting surveys, usually through interviews or written questionnaires. Even though questionnaires are not always filled out truthfully and their validity may be relative, the data obtained are important to form hypothesis that can be tested later on. Another problem with observation checklists and with surveys is that items have to be decided in advance and sometimes some irrelevant aspects are not included and no feedback is obtained. This problem can be solved with very open questions where the surveyed subjects can mention anything that seems relevant to them. This desire not to prejudge the importance of potential relevant events has led some researchers to explore the procedures and techniques of ethnography as a viable approach to classroom research (see van Lier 1988,  Watson-Gegeo, 1988, Erickson 1981, Green and Wallet 1981). For example, van Lier (1988) considers ethnographic techniques the most suitable to study classroom events. He states that to understand what happens in classrooms, we must ask about the meanings that the participants give to the successive classroom phenomena.

 

Observation in the classroom and ELT research began to be used in the 1960s in connection with teacher training to provide the trainees with feedback about their performance in class during their teaching practice or after the short periods of microteaching with their college peers. In most cases, the main objective was to investigate what constituted effective teaching in order to select those reflective teaching techniques and become competent to use them in the future, in similar situations. But the teaching and learning processes have proved to be so complex and unpredictable that this technological conception of education has taken other directions, as we will see in the next section.

 

2.4.2.1 Interaction analysis

 

One of the earliest and most popular observation model was developed by Flanders (1970). He developed a list of categories of teacher and learner behaviour associated with successful teaching. His taxonomy is called FIAC (Flanders’ Interaction Analysis Categories) and contained the following groups  of categories (see Appendix  1.1):

 

TEACHER TALK

Indirect influence

Accepts feeling

Praises or encourages

Accept or uses ideas of student

Asks questions

Direct influence

Lecturing

Giving directions

Criticizing or justifying authority

 

STUDENT TALK

Student-talk response

Student talk-initiation

Silence or confusion

 

 

To codify the behaviour used during a teaching unit the observer marks every time a different category. When the same category is repeated, the observer records this category every three seconds. All the tallies are registered in a matrix which thus show a graphic picture of the lesson. By analysing the matrix, some teaching patterns ca be discovered.

 

Flanders model was soon criticized by those who considered classroom behaviour too complex to be reduced to ten categories and other models soon emerged. It was Moskowwitz (1976, 1971) who produced the most widely used modification of Flanders’ model. She called her taxonomy “FLint” (Foreign Language Interaction) and included the following categories (see section 1.4.1):

 

 

TEACHER TALK

Indirect influence

Deals with feelings

Praises or encourages

Jokes

Uses ideas of students

Repeats student response verbatim

Asks questions

Direct influence

Gives information

Corrects without rejection

Gives directions

Directs pattern Drills

Criticizes students behaviour

Criticizes students response

 

 

STUDENT TALK

Student response, specific

Student response, choral

Student response, open-ended or student initiated

Silence

Confusion

Laughter

 

 

Moskowitz’s taxonomy was used as a research tool and as a feedback instrument in teacher training. She trained student teachers to analyse their teaching behaviour using her taxonomy for them to have a more objective picture of themselves as potential teachers. It was also used as a reference of what constitutes good and efficient language teaching.

 

Fanselow (1977) made another important contribution with his FOCUS (Foci for Observing Communication Used in Settings). It was also an observation schedule developed for language teacher training but it is applicable for classroom research. As we can see in the following table, Fanselow’s model does not have separate categories for teachers and learners, but instead has general categories which define five characteristic of communication in settings (see section 1.4.1):

 

 

-      Who communicates?

-      What is the pedagogical purpose of the communication?

-      What mediums are used to communicate?

-      How are the mediums used to communicate areas of content?

-      What areas of content are communicated?

 

 

The model we propose  (Madrid, 1998a, 1998b)  is related to the  communicative approach, too,  but it is primarily based on the communicative competence construct, defined as the integration of  five basic sub-competences (see Canale, 1983; Savignon, 1983; Kohonen et al. (1985), Bachman, 1990):

a)      Linguistic or grammatical subcompetence

b)      Sociolinguistic or illocutionary subcompetence

c)      Sociocultural subcompetence

d)      Discourse competence

e)      Strategic (and procedural) competence

 

So, we aim to analyse the attention paid to these subcompetences in the classroom; that is, the time that the teacher and the students spend developing each one, in isolation or integrated in different

teaching and learning situations. These sub-competencies include the categories presented below that must be observed and codified in a systematic way every minute (see instructions in Appendix 1.8):

 

 

a)       Linguistic or grammatical subcompetence:

-        Grammar

-        Lexicon (vocabulary)

-        Phonetics

 

 

b)       Sociolinguistic or illocutionary subcompetence

-        Communcative functions

-        Appropriateness of grammatical forms

 

 

c)       Sociocultural subcompetence

-        Cultural concepts, attitudes, values, ...

 

 

d)       Discourse subcompetence

-        Working with texts: structure, coherence, cohesiveness, ...

 

 

e)       Strategic competence

-        Learning skills and procedures: listening, speaking, reading and writing

-        Mental operations which regulate the learning processes: cognitive, metacognitive, affective, communicative, ...

 

 

 

Obviously, the strategic subcompetence is of a psychological nature and cannot be analysed without the learner cooperation. That is, an additional information coming from the learner is necessary.  The following model is suggested for data collection related to a) block 1 about the communicative competence components and b) block 2  about the structural vs. the discursive paradigm (see Appendix 2.8 for a fully description):

For the analysis of the classroom teaching and learning activities related to the  receptive and productive skills and procedures, we propose the following model:

 

The model that we propose (fully described in appendix 2.8) is also based on the process-product paradigm (see Section 2.3) which seeks direct and indirect relations between the teaching and learning process and the final results that the student  obtains (see Dunkin and Biddle, 1974). In this sense, we also aim to analyse the English class to study the connection between what is done in class and the effect on the students’ communicative competence. In order to study that connection, the taxonomy presented in Appendix 2.8 is proposed. In addition to that, suitable tests and evaluation techniques must be administered to obtain the information needed about the final outcomes and have thus some evidence about  the relationship between the learning process and the final product (outcomes).

 

 

2.4.2.2 Discourse analysis

 

The power emergence of discourse analysis studies as a field of linguistic enquiry has led to classroom research models also based on the analysis of classroom discourse. An example of these types of contributions can be the COLT system proposed by Allen, Frölich and Spada (1985). The COLT model (Communicative Orientation of Language Teaching) was developed to distinguish communicative language teaching classrooms from those more form-focused. The categories aimed to measure the extend to which an instructional treatment was communicatively oriented and included the following headings:

 

 

Part A: Classroom events

I.        Activity

II.      Participant organization

III.    Content

IV.    Student modality

V.      Materials

 

 

Part B: Communicative features

I.        Use of target language

II.      Information gap

III.    Sustained speech

IV.    Reaction to code or message

V.      Incorporation of preceding utterances

VI.    Discourse initiation

VII.  Relative restriction of linguistic form

 

 

 

Sinclair and Coulthard (1975) also analysed the discourse of some L1 British elementary schools classrooms. In their model, the discourse level is comprised of five ranks (lesson, transation, exchange, move, act). The pedagogical move consists of various structures, which are realized by “acts” having a specific discourse function. For example, an “opening” move consists of a head act, either elicitation, directive, informative or check. These acts resemble the concept of Searle’s speech acts (1969), illocutionary acts or communicative functions,  widely employed to analyse the functional value of  language use. Sinclair and Coulthard include the following group of categories (see Sinclair and Coulthard, 1975: 24-27):

 

 

 

Rank 1: Lesson

                Elements of structure, structures, classes

Rank 2: Transaction

                Elements of structure, structures, classes of exchange

Rank 3: Exchange (Boundary); Exchange (Teaching)

                Elements of structure, structures, classes of move

Rank 4: Move (Opening)

              Elements of structure, structures, classes of act

 

           

 

 

For second language classroom research Chaudron offers the following list of categories (1988:45):

 

 

 

STRUCTURAL UNITS

utterance: a string of speech by one speaker under a single intonation contour, arid preceded and followed by another speaker's speech, or a pause of more than x seconds

turn: any speaker's sequence of utterances bounded by another speaker's speech

T-unit:                any syntactic main clause and its associated subordinate clauses

Communication unit:  an independent grammatical predication; the same as a T-unit, except that in   oral language, elliptical answers to questions also constitute complete predications

fragment: any utterance which does not constitute a completed proposition (i.e., with explicit subject and verb)

 

FUNCTIONAL UNITS

repetition: an exact repeating of a previous string of speech (either partial or full, and either a self- or other-repetition)

expansion:  a partial or full repetition which modifies some portion of a previous string of speech by adding Syntactic or semantic information

clarification request: a request for further information from an interlocutor about a previous utterance

comprehension check:   the speaker's query of the interlocutor(s) as to whether or not they have understood the previous speaker utterance(s)

confirmation check:  the speaker's query as 10 whether or not the speaker's (expressed) understanding of the interlocutor's meaning is correct

repair: an attempt by a speaker to alter or rectify a previous utterance which was in some way lacking in clarity or correctness (either self- or other-directed);

model: a type of prompt by a speaker (usually a teacher) intended to elicit an exact imitation or to serve as an exemplary response 10 an elicitation

 

 

 

Discourse analysis has contributed to a better awareness of the internal structure and functional purpose of the verbal classroom interaction and it has been greatly influenced by the ethnographic research tradition

 

 

2.4.3    ETHNOGRAPHY

 

Given the shortcomings of scientific research, which we have discussed above, it is not surprising to find that theorists began to pose alternative research paradigms and used a naturalistic enquiry for their research. In this case, the researcher tries not to intervene in the research setting and does not try to control naturally occurring events. Quite the contrary, they purposefully try not to influence the normally occurring patterns of instruction and interaction because they wish to describe and understand these processes rather than to test specific hypothesis about cause-and effect relationships. While it may include comparison groups and can involve the collection of both quantitative and qualitative data, it does not use the experimental concept of "treatment". Nor does the researcher create special groups for the purposes of experimentation and observation. Instead, naturally occurring groups become the focal point (Watson-Gegeo, 1988). For this reason, regularly scheduled classes are likely sites in which to use the naturalistic enquiry approach.

 

Hammersley and Atkinson present a summary of views which can help us to understand the wide scope of ethnography (1983: 1-2):

 

 

Ethnography is:

-      the elicitation of cultural knowledge (Spradley 1980)

-      the detailed investigation of patterns of social interaction (Gumperz 1981)

-      holistic analysis of societies (Lutz 1981)

-      essentially descriptive, a form of story telling (Walker 1981)

-      the development and testing of theory (Glaser and Strauss 1967; Denzin 1978)

-      one social research method, drawing on a wide range of sources of information (flammersley and Atkinson 1983)

 

 

 

The growth of the discipline of ethnography has had a profound influence on the acceptance of an alternative research paradigm. Ethnography has its roots in anthropology and sociology, and it revolves around two key ideas:

 

-         context is seen to have an important influence on behaviour, therefore research needs to always be carried out in context, not under 'laboratory' conditions.

-          the subjective perceptions and belief systems of both researchers and subjects need to be taken into account in any research. This is because 'objectivity' is an impossibility, given that there are no external 'Truths' or objective reality to be discovered. Thus research focuses on the cultural meanings revealed by the behaviour of the subjects under study.

 

Thus ethnography involves "the study of the culture/characteristics of a group in real-world rather than laboratory settings. The researcher makes no attempt to isolate or manipulate the phenomena under investigation, and insights and generalisations emerge from close contact with the data rather than from a theory of language learning and use." (Nunan 1992:55).

 

Obviously these ideas are akin to the idea of the reflective practitioner, and have resulted in the development of a kind of classroom research called "action research", which we will examine in detail later in this unit. But many observational studies have also followed the ethnographic methodology (see van Lier, 1988).

 

Mehan (1979) has also criticized quantitative oriented surveys, coding and experimental studies  and proposes what he calls a constitutive ethnography with the following basic charecteristics:

 

 

1.        Retrievability of the data. Ethnograpic techniques such us video and audio-tapes provide a record of the data studied that can be used to re-examine and re-interpret them.

2.        Comprehensive data treatment. Its comprehensive approach to classroom observation and analyses allows the study of each and every intance of teacher-student interaction.

3.        Convergence between researcher’s and participants’ perspectives. The structures and actions described reflect the participants view, how they perceive classroom events.

4.        Interactional level of analysis. The goal is in Mehan’s words to ...

locate the organizing machinery of classroom lessons in the interaction, ... the words in the actions of the participants, ..

.

 

 

A major procedure of ethnographic work is the detailed analysis of recorded data. For any ethnographic study based on classroom videotaped data, Erickson and Shultz’s stages are very useful (1981:153-7).

 

 

 

Stage 1. Global viewing: taking sparse notes to index the tapes and note transitions between occasions of interest.

Stage 2. Choice of specific occasions of interest for more detailed analysis. Selected occasions are copied onto copy tapes. Occasions are timed, and junctures described in detail. Participants may be asked to attend a viewing session and give their emic views of the occasion.

Stage 3. Specification of differences in the transitions or junctures, with specific attention paid to nonverbal contextualization cues.

Stage 4. Detailed description of the participation structures between junctures.

Stage 5. Construction of a model showing the principles of social organization underlying the surface form of communication behavior in interaction.

Stage 6. Establishing the generalizability of the structures analysed.

 

(Summarized from: Erickson and Shultz 1981, p.153-7, in van Lier, 1988:65)

 

 

 

 

2.4.4    ACTION RESEARCH

 

One of the new buzzwords in ELT is that of "action research". Martin Parrott  provides the following definition:

 

Action research is not so much something that we do in addition to our teaching as something that we integrate into it. In many ways it is a state of mind - it is a scepticism about assumptions and a willingness to put everything to the test. It is something which should take very little time and which does not necessarily need to be made public. It is a way of ensuring that we continue to learn even as we teach. It helps stave off staleness and routine. (1996:3)

 

Action research is thus research on the smallest scale: that of the individual teacher in his/her classroom carrying out investigations into teaching and learning in very specific contexts with very specific groups of learners. Action research is what the reflective practitioner actually does in the classroom. It  is typically direct intervention with only limited possibilities for control. Although it can take on many forms, action research in classrooms basically involves taking an action and systematically observing what follows.

Action research has been defined as a form of self-reflective enquiry undertaken by participants in social situations in order to improve the rationality and justice of their own social or educational practices, as well as their understanding of these practices and the situations in which these practices are carried out" (Kemmis and Henry 1989:2). It is characterised as being a participatory, self-reflective and collaborative approach to research.

 

The fundamental characteristics of action-research according to Elliott, one of its pioneers,  are the following (1990):

-         It analyses the human actions and social situations that students and teachers experience.

-         It uses an exploratory approach

-         It aims to explain what happens in the classroom in relation to specific teaching contents.

-         It interprets the different classroom events from the point of view of those who  take part in each situation; that is, it involves teachers and students: their beliefs, values, intentions, decisions, ...

-         It uses a very direct and simple language to explain the classroom situations that are analysed far from the technical and specialised language used by conventional research.

 

Kemmis and McTaggart (1988) argue that action research has three defining characteristics: firstly that it is carried out by classroom teachers rather by than outside researchers; secondly, it is collaborative; thirdly, it is aimed at changing things:

 

A distinctive feature of action research is that those affected by planned changes have the primary responsibility for deciding on courses of critically informed action which seem likely to lead to improvement, and for evaluating the results of strategies tried out in practice. Action research is a group activity. (1988:6).

 

Thus for Kemmis and McTaggart the essential impetus for carrying out action research is to change the system. Cohen and Manion (1985) add that action research is primarily situational, as it is concerned with the solution of problems in a specific context.

 

However, it is now widely accepted that action research does not necessarily have to be collaborative, nor does it have to result in change. A single teacher, working on identifying aspects of his/her teaching practice in the classroom, can also be considered to be undertaking a perfectly valid piece of action research.

 

 

2.4.4.1 Stages  in action research

 

There are four classic developmental phases of action research:

 

Phase 1: Develop a plan of action to a) improve what is already happening or

              b) identify and examine a "puzzle" or problem area in your teaching.

Phase 2: Act to implement the plan.

Phase 3: Observe the effects of action in the context in which it occurs.

Phase 4: Reflect on these effects.

 

Nevertheless, other authors establish the following eight stages (Cohenand Manion, 1985):

 

 

Stage 1. The identification, evaluation and formulation of the problem.

Stage 2. Preliminary discussion and negotiations amongst interested parties – teachers, advisers, researchers, sponsors – culminating in a draft proposal.

Stage 3. Review of research literature and comparable studies.

Stage 4. Restatement of the problem, or formulation of a hypothesis; explicit discussion of the assumptions underlying the project.

Stage 5. Selection of research procedures, allocation of resources, choice of materials and methods, etc.

Stage 6. Choice of evaluation procedures - bearing in mind that evaluation will be continuous.

Stage 7. The implementation of the project itself, including data collection and analysis, monitoring and feedback.

Stage 8. The interpretation of the data; inferences to be drawn; overall project evaluation.

 

 

 

Nunan describes an action research project which has a slightly broader scope (see Appendix 2). According to Nunan, the key points to note about action research are that, firstly, the research is initiated by the teacher him/herself and is derived from a real problem in the classroom which needs to be confronted. Secondly, real objective data is collected (in Nunan's project in Appendix 2, this data takes the form of classroom interactions and learner language). Thirdly, the results of the research project are disseminated. Finally, the project takes the form of an ongoing cycle in which the teacher reflects on, returns to, and extends the initial inquiry. (1992:18-19).

 

Action research is thus the putting into action of the reflective model of research which we discussed earlier, and which is summarised in the following diagram:

 

Action research can be used by the classroom teacher for a variety of purposes: it can be used as a way of learning about our learners, and about our teaching and its effectiveness, and as a way of monitoring and evaluating innovation. We will look at each of these areas separately.

 

A way of learning about our learners:

By asking about and taking learners' attitudes and preferences into account, we can reflect on and evaluate our teaching. Ways of doing this include: a simple show of hands (The teacher simply poses a series of questions - eg. "Who wants to be corrected more? Who wants only the teacher to correct them? Who likes mistakes to be corrected by other students?"etc) - and students show preferences by putting up their hands; questionnaires, in the mother tongue if necessary; teacher/learner diaries or journals can be used for feedback on specific issues; brief teacher/student interviews; and so on.

 

Reflecting and analysing our teaching:

There is a great deal we can learn about the way we teach through simple observation of different kinds. For example, we can record or video our lessons and listen to or watch the recording later; peer observation; and so on. (See Subject *). Here a test-teach-test approach might be appropriate. For example, the teacher might decide to 'test' the learners' ability to recall vocabulary after teaching it in a variety of ways, with various classes. The important point here is that evidence is unlikely to be conclusive: an experiment of this type could well lead to the conclusion that no one method is better than any other, and that certain students may learn better than others depending on the approach taken. In this case, the teacher may conclude that a variety of approaches is necessary to cater to a variety of learner styles.

 

In a more general sense, action research often has a specific and immediate outcome which can be directly related to practice in the teacher's own context. As it does not make any claims for universal relevance, the research methods employed in action research can be more free-ranging and less rigorous than those employed in conventional research. By allowing for discussion and the sharing of ideas and experience among colleagues, it can open up dialogue on issues that worry teachers, and it can help to kindle and maintain enthusiasm about our classroom practice.

 

 

A way of monitoring and evaluating innovation: Simple but systematic evaluation of innovation can help teachers to incorporate principled change into their teaching. What is needed is to start evaluating before the innovation is introduced, and to make the evaluation indirect.

 

 

2.4.4.2 Criticisms of action research

 

Posing as it does a rather more flexible, reflective model of research, action research has predictably come in for a certain amount of criticism. This is inevitable, as it reflects an entirely different view of the status of knowledge to the more traditional scientific research paradigm, which we discussed earlier.

 

The issues of reliability and validity are key in talking about and evaluating research. Reliability refers to the consistency of results obtained from research

- how consistent is the collection of data within a piece of research? Reliability also refers to replicability - can the same piece of research be carried out with another group of learners and provide similar results? Validity seeks to establish to what extent the research really investigates what the researcher

wants it to - are the research tools used consistent with what the researcher is trying to research?

 

Criticisms of action research have centred around its lack of vigorous attention to both reliability and validity, particularly external validity or replicability (the extent to which independent researchers can reproduce a study and obtain results similar to those obtained in the original study).

 

However, it has been pointed out that the very nature of action research precludes such concern over issues such as external validity because "in many cases practitioners are less concerned with generating generalisable knowledge than with solving pressing problems associated with their own particular workplace. While such (action research) activities therefore fulfil a professional development function, I still believe that if they address questions of interest to other practitioners, if they generate data, and if they contain analysis and interpretation, then they qualify as research." (Nunan 1992:18-19).

 

Another criticism of action research lies in the fact that it is unable to provide 'facts' or universal truths about language teaching, as its scope is limited. However, as this criticism lies in a different perception of the status of knowledge to that proposed by action research, as discussed earlier, it is enough to assert that "the aim of action research is not to arrive at universal truths but only to learn more about ourselves (at the moment), our teaching (at the moment), our learners (at the moment) and their learning (at the moment)." (Parrott 1996:6).

 

2.5  TEACHERS' BELIEFS

 

Individual teachers exhibit certain typical behaviour traits in the classroom. We all know that each teacher is different, and will react differently in any given classroom situation, but what makes that difference? MacLeod and MacIntyre (1977:260) suggest that individual teachers are able to make very fast, complex professional decisions on the job due to having a limited number of deep underlying "conceptual schemata" or 'constructs' of what appropriate professional action might be. We could define 'construct' as a broad term used to include a group of related concepts such as beliefs, ideas, attitudes, etc. all of which shape our behaviour in various typical or consistent ways.

 

Research studies about teachers’ beliefs have increased dramatically for the last twenty years. The general aim has been to find out what happens in the classroom in order to improve the teaching and learning situations. This type of research provides ideas and principles which can be undertaken as hypothesis to be checked in other contexts and also gives evidence about  the variety of  pedagogical ideas and beliefs that teachers have and  their connection with different modes of performing in the classroom.

 

Floden and Klinzing (1990) have found a very valuable relationship between the teacher’s beliefs and his/her initial education, which becomes an important source of  ideas, beliefs and models that are applied later to different classroom situations. De Vicente has also highlighted the importance of this paradigm in order to train teachers within the reflective-in-action-paradigm.  Given that the research studies that conform this paradigm are based on case studies and report about very personal and sometimes unique situations, they often derive into reflection-in-action processes and promote a critic reflection of teaching. De Vicente has differentiated three basic stages:

a)      Cognitive stage: when these studies focus on the use of the cognitive skills that teachers make when preparing their curricular activities, implementing them and making decisions in  the classroom.

b)      Critical stage: when teachers report about their beliefs, opinions, values and experiences critically.

c)      Interpretative stage: when teachers interpret what happens in the classroom and narrate classroom events.

 

Nunan (1991:50) gives a clear example of how mental constructs could operate in a specific classroom situation:

 

Thus, for example, language teachers who have detected a learner making a pronunciation error in the target language will usually deal with it in a way that is at least partly determined by mental constructs... (ie. in the light of their views on the importance of fluency as opposed to accuracy, on the nature of the cause of the error; appropriate ways of dealing with such errors, the importance of pronunciation errors as opposed to other kinds of errors, and so on). Of course their reaction may also be influenced by aspects of the incident specific to this particular situation: their relationship with a particular  learner; whether they are feeling particulariy tired or irritable at the time, etc. But, in general terms, if they are professionals, they will exhibit an overall consistency of response in this area which will relate to the repertoire of mental constructs which they have developed.

 

 

Wittrock (1990) has designed a model to explain the relationship between teachers’ thought and its effect on the student’s behaviour and achievement that we present in fig. 2 with some changes:

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 


Fig. 3: Components of the teacher’s thought paradigm (Wittrock)

 

As we can see in fig. 3, the teacher’s thought/beliefs operate following three stages which cyclically conform each teacher’s mental system:

a)      Pre-action thought / beliefs, which include the previous ideas, preconceptions and initial opinions we have at the moment we are preparing and planning the teaching units.

b)      Interactive thought / beliefs, which operates while implementing what we have planned, when we teach what we have prepared in class and a variety of interactive processes take place.

c)      Post-action thought / beliefs, these include the ideas we have formed after we have finished our teaching, as a result of the unpredictable events that may have happened in class, after we have observed the students’ reaction and the outcomes obtained. In this 3rd stage, our preconceptions and initial ideas may have changed when confronted with reality or may be confirmed and consolidated.

Stage 1: Planning stage: lesson planning, preconceptions, pre-action thought

What curricular ideas underlie this lesson planning? What kind of preconceptions are taken for granted when this teacher designed the unit in this way?

Stage 2: Implementation: teaching performance, learning processes, interactive thought

 

If a group of students is available, try to implement this lesson planning. If you do not have any students, think about the students’ reaction when receiving this teaching and the ideas and concepts that you would question and revise as a result of the teaching and learning process.

 

Stage 3:  Post-action thought / beliefs: after the students’ reaction and the questions that emerged and were raised in the second stage, what initial ideas have you changed? What modifications are necessary for the future? What theoretical aspects do you think are still weak and need more research and practical verification to be fully accepteded?

______________________________________________________________________________

 

The sources of mental constructs are many, and come from both received and experiential knowledge, as well as from personality factors, social factors and cultural factors. Even novice teachers, with no direct classroom experience or formal training will have mental constructs about teaching, derived primarily from what Gutiérrez Almarza calls their "apprenticeship of observation" (1996:51). By this she means their experiences of being taught and of watching teachers teach.

 

Obviously action research can provide a tool for teachers to discover and examine their own individual mental constructs.

 

2.6       DATA COLLECTION

 

Other  steps in classroom research are to decide what we want to investigate, in which way and how to approach it. On the other hands, the sort of data we can use to analyse classroom language can be obtained from two different sources:

-     Using reports of earlier classroom research

-     Watching language learners

 

 With the first approach we can read different types of research and replicate those studies that seem more interesting for our purposes. In this case,  it is acceptable to use research plans, questionnaires or observation instruments from published documents to conduct our own research, provided we give credit to the original authors. From the point of view of research, the replication of existing studies is an important way to go a step further as it is part of the refinement process.

 

In any case it is important to bear in mind, if we want to adopt this view, that some studies are not probably as well designed as we might believe. Accordingly it is usually more appropriate to talk of follow-up studies rather than of exact replications. This way of approaching and analysing language data can be advisable if we are not yet working with a group of learners.

 

Watching language learners in a classroom context can definitely be a good source of ideas for investigations or be based on previous research and/or theory. Regarding this latter point, Allright and Bailey (1991) have underlined what there is a dynamic tension between these two opposing points of view on how to determine the topic of an investigation. The first position, which is associated with experimental science holds that a researcher should decide in advance what to investigate, on the basis of predictions generated by theory. In the second view, that more commonly associated, the research questions and hypotheses arise from the data that are collected.

 

Some theorists would argue that any hypothesis or research question ought to come directly from a theory which makes predictions that can be empirically tested by some sort of classroom investigation.

 

However, as it has been suggested, there are two problems with this way of approaching research. On the one hand, the theories of language learning do not always lend themselves to making directly testable predictions. In addition, some researchers (van Lier, 1988) consider that classroom lessons are such complex affairs that it is virtually impossible ever to control the number of different variables that could bias the results of any attempt to test a particular theory-driven prediction.

 

On the other hand, the second problem with putting theory first is that it misses the point that theories themselves have to come from somewhere.

 

But, as Allwright and Bailey have put it, classroom research does not always have to concern itself so directly with theories at all, whether to test them or to illuminate them. Instead, classroom research can be directed at trying to understand and deal with the immediate practical problerns facing teachers and learners. The term for this sort of work, aimed as it is at investigating and dealing with immediate practical problems, is "action research".

In any case, whether we begin from the data-first or the theory first position, it is important to have a specific issue in mind, a particular problem to think about, because there is a strong risk of wasting a lot of our own and everyone else's time if we begin a research project with no clear idea of what we are going to do.

 

Given these opposing viewpoints on the research sequence (data-first versus theory-first), it would probably be best to start off with at least a general issue we want to investigate, and to use your thought about that issue to help you to decide what sorts of data you will need.

Another aspect of the decision about what to investigate is the understandable temptation to look at the most visible things only, the things that are easiest to observe, to record, and to count.

 

Two problems arise in this area of deciding what to investigate. First,  the overall picture we have of classroom language learning from research so far is already distorted by this bias towards the visible.

 

Second, the bias towards the easy things to investigate is a luxury that action researchers at least cannot usually afford.

 

 

2.6.1        Interviews

 

The objective of interviews is to obtain information by actually talking to the subjects under study. Sometimes, it is also necessary to ask the students questions about the classrooms events either in an individual face to face situation or to the whole group of students. Perhaps the two main disadvatages are that individual interviews are a) time consuming and b) they often introduce subjective and biased information, given that interviewees often say what  they think that will please the interviewer (see Appendix 2.9).

 

Besides that, certain covert variables (e.g. attitudes, prejudices, interests, needs analysis, learning strategies, motivation, etc.) cannot be fully studied unless we interview the subjects involved in the research work and collect information about their beliefs, feelings and opinions. According to the degree of explicitness and structure, interviews can be “open” and “semi-open or “semi-structured”.

a)      Open interviews provide the interviewee with very open questions which allow a great freedom of expression to give the answers. Very often, they are carried out through informal talks about the topics under study.

b)      Semi-open interviews provide some core questions predetermined in advance but the subjects  interviewed still feel quite free to answer them.

c)      Semi-structured interviews consists of specific and defined questions determined beforehand but tyhey allow some ellaboration in the questions and answers.

d)      Structured  interviews include very specific close questions that require very specific answers.

 

In general, semi-structured and structured interviews need some kind of interview schedule, checklist or questionnaire which presents the questions to be asked and the topics to be discussed with some space for the interviewer to write down the answers.

 

 

2.6.2    Questionnaires

 

Questionnaires are printed and used for data collection. They include questions or statements that are responded in an anonymous way (see Appendix 2.10). They are similar to interviews in the type of data that are provided but in questionnaires the questions/statements are in a written form whereas in interviews they are oral. The use of questionnaires has also some advantages:

a)      They can be administered to large groups of subjects

b)      The data provided tend to be quite uniform and standard

c)      If they are applied to groups of subjects at the same time, the data collected can be very accurate.

But there are also some disadvantages:

a)      If they are sent home to be answered the response rate is usually very low and that may affect validity and reliability.

b)      Sometimes, the data provided can be very subjective and need to be contrasted and checked in other situations

 

Questionnaires, the same as interviews,  can also be open, semi-open, semi-structured and structured. Quite frequently, structured questionnaires use the Likert scale to grade the statements from 1 to 5 (e.g.: 1 = never, 2 = seldom 3 = sometimes, 4 = often, 5 = always). The semantic differential is another technique which grades the items with a bipolar scale (e.g. bad/good; high/low, ...)

 

2.6.3    Case study

 

This type of research  focuses on one or a few individuals. In most cases, these studies are longitudinal, that is, they follow the individuals for a long period of time. Many case studies have centred, for example,  on the development of one subject’s (or a few ) interlanguage for a few years in order to describe the process. For example, Ellis work on the grammatical and semantic development of three subjects (1984) and his recommendations about the importance of the learner’s initiative in interaction for second language acquisition. This type of research is very important to draw up hypothesis that need to be tested later on with more representative samples.

 

 

2.6.4    Diaries

 

Even though diaries can be anecdotal and subjective, they can provide very important clues about what the learners feel and how they process information. There are many mental operations and strategies that play a crucial role in language learning and that cannot be observed and studied unless the student reports about them. This technique is very useful to explore the learning strategies that the students use in different situations, especially when they receive  metacognitive instruction and we want to know what

kind of effect has it got on the student learning.

 

 

 

2.7    PROBLEMS IN COLLECTING  DATA

 

In the early days of classroom research observation instruments were used which focused primarily on the teacher's behaviour. But as language classroom research has become more deeply involved with issues in language learning, a clearer focus on the learners, and on the interaction among learners and teachers, has superseded the earlier emphasis on teacher behaviour. One result of this shift in focus has been a decrease in the use of observation schedules and an increase in the use of discourse analysis of transcribed data.

 

2.7.1 Discourse analysis and transcription

 

Discourse analysis" refers to a variety of procedures for examining chunks of language, whether spoken or written (Allwright and Bailey, 1991).

In the case of classroom research, discourse analysis usually involves the analysis of spoken language as it is used in classrooms among teachers and learners. Van Lier (1988:122) describes it as "an analysis of the processes of interaction by means of a close examination of audiovisual records of interaction".

The focus on lengths of oral discourse in classroom interaction leads us naturally to units of analysis which are different from the concepts of sentence, clause, or phrase, as these terms are used in syntactic analysis. Instead, discourse analysts, who are interested in the way talk is structured, have investigated concepts such as utterances, repair strategies, topic nomination and turns.

 

Discourse analysts typically use transcripts and audiotaped or videotaped interactions as their data base. Some use transcripts and accompanying videotapes in order to document the nonverbal channel of communications (see Appendix 2.6).

 

Transcripts vary widely in their level of technical complexity. They may use standard ortography or detailed phonetic transcription of speech, depending on the research goal.

 

 

2.7.2      Triangulation: the value of multiple perspectives

 

Anthropologists have borrowed this term from land surveying to suggest that at least two perspectives are necessary if an accurate picture of a particular phenomenon is to be obtained. Triangulation can take several different forms (Denzin, 1970:472).

 

a)      One of them is data triangulation, which means using a variety of sampling strategies, for example, data related to the time and social situations collected in different occasions to ensure that the objectives proposed  are studied in more than one way.

b)      Another is investigator triangulation, in which more than one observer contributes to the findings to gain more reliability.

c)      Methodological triangulation refers to using different methods (for example, observation, analysis of transcripts and self-report surveys) to collect the data.

d)      Finally, theoretical triangulation demands that the researchers approach the data analysis with more than one perspective on possible interpretations.

 

 The combination of multiple methods, data types, observers and theories in the same research study is called multiple triangulation (Denzin 1970:472).

 

2.7.3 Reliability

 

This term refers to the fact that the research procedures must be consistent, both over time and across a variety of people who might use them. It applies to both the data collection and data analysis of classroom research.

 

One area of obvious concern about reliability is in situations where more than one observer is involved in trying to count or code the same things.

 

The degree of reliability between observers can in fact be calculated, and of course steps can be taken to train observers to improve their "inter-observer agreement" figures. In general, before going ahead with the coding of a large corpus of data, classroom researchers strive for at least 85% agreement among observers and raters. This figure is called "inter-observer agreement" or "inter-rater reliability". Another matter for concern is the percentage of "intra­-observer agreement"- that is, the extent to which a single observer or coder, working with the same data, codes or categorises the data consistently after a lapse or time. The figures range from 0 to 1.00, with decimals nearer to 1.00 representing greater consistency in the observers' use of the categories.

 

2.7.4 Validity

 

As a technical construct in experimental studies, validity takes two important forms. First, there is the notion of "internal validity". A study is said to have internal validity if the outcomes of the experiment can be directly and unambiguously attributed to the treatment applied to the experimental group, rather than to uncontrolled factors.

 

Internal validity relates to the extent to which the results of an experimental study can be reliably and unambiguously related to the treatment which was implemented.

Chaudron (1 988b) has discussed 3 types of validation which relate to classroom research:

 

-         Construct validation: the construct is verifiable and can be "captured" through various measurement procedures.

-         Criterion-related validity: some form of measurement is used to measure a trait along with another form.

-         Treatment validation: the treatment was in fact implemented and that is identifiably different from whatever it was being compared with.

 

According to Seliger and Shohamy (1989:190) the information needed for determining the quality of data collection procedures is the following:

 

         

          Technique                                                The information it provides

            

             Reliability                whether the scores are accurate

             Test-retest                whether the scores are stable overtime

             Inter-rater                  whether there is agreement among judges about the score assigned

             Intra-rater                  whether a rater will assign the same score after some time has elapsed

             Parallel form              whether two similar instruments supposed to measure the same thing actually do

             Internal                      whether the test items are related to one another and

             consistency              measure the same thing

             Validity                  whether it measures what it is supposed to measure

             Content                  whether the procedure represents accurately the content it is supposed to          measure

             Concurrent            whether it correlates well with a different type of instrument which is  suppose to                                           measure the same thing

             Predictive               whether the measure can predict accurately a certain future behavior

             Construct               whether it represents accurately the theory of the variable which it measures

             Item analysis        whether the items and questions which appear on the instrument are difficult or easy, and whether they discriminate among the subjects of the research.

                

2.7.5 Generalisability

 

The other form of validity in experimental research, to set against the internal kind, is of course "external validity" or generalisability. As the name implies, the issue involves the extent to which the findings of a study can be generalised, or applied, to other (external) situations.

Generalisability is connected to the concepts of "population" and "sample". In experimental science, the population is the entire group of subjects of interest -in our case, typical learners-. The "sample" is that smaller group which is actually studied by the researcher.

 

It is important to note that reliability and validity are intertwined1 and importantly so. If the results of an experiment are not reliable they cannot be applied to other contexts. Furthermore, in experimental research terms, there can be no "external validity" (generalisability) without "internal validity". The reasoning behind these maxims is that the findings of an experiment (if there are problems in measurement, for example) then there is no point in trying to apply that treatment in other settings.

 

We will naturally want to know whether or not the findings would hold true with our own learners. For this reason, it is important to know something about the background of the sample of learners investigated in the study.

 

When the results of the study are presented in numerical form, it is common to see "mean scores", which are simply mathematical averages, and "standard deviations". The standard deviation is a statistic measure which indicates how much variety there is in a group of scores. It is a measure of the average distance of the scores in a group of scores from the average score of that group (see Brown, 1988).

 

Experimental research aims at investigating a situation without changing it in any way other than the treatment, while action research deliberately aims at bringing about changes for the better in the specific situation being investigated. Another major difference between experimental research and action research -by definition- can afford to confine itself to looking for local situations to local problems, whereas experimental researchers are committed to trying to develop the sort of understanding that will apply much more globally.

 

For this reason, the issue of generalisability is not so crucial in the tradition of action research. In naturalistic enquiry similarly, and again in contrast to experimental research, generalisability is not always such a primary focus.

 

In language classroom research, Van Lier has argued that generalisability cannot be a major goal because -the first concern must be to analyse the data as they are rather than to compare them to other data to see how similar they are" (1988:2)

 

But then the whole issue of generalisabilty looks very different to anyone doing research in naturalistic tradition. Instead of claiming that whatever has been discovered must be true of people in general, a naturalistic enquirer will claim that whatever understanding has been gained by an in-depth study of a real-life classroom may illuminate issues for other people.

 

 

2.8         ANALYSING THE DATA

 

Data analysis is the final stage of research. It implies organising the data collected in order to study to what extent  the objectives that we predetermined have been achieved,  to check if the hypothesis formulated can be confirmed or simply to draw conclusions from an ethnographic study. This analysis leads to the final conclusions of the research.

 

In this stage, as with the previous phases, there is  a variety of techniques that we can use and the results will depend on the type of analysis. But it is important to notice that there must be a relation between the nature of the research problem, the research method, the variables we want to control, the tools used to control the variables and the procedures chosen to analyse  the data collected. Some data analysis techniques will be more appropriate for quantitative research, while others will be more appropriate for qualitative research.  Some of the well known handbooks which offer a gool deal of practical  information for data analysis are Seliger and Shohami, (1989); Brown, (1988); Tuckman, (1978), just to give a few examples.

 

 

2.8.1      Qualitative research data

 

In qualitative research, where the data have been collected through observations, interviews, diaries, or any other qualitative procedure, the information is gathered in recordings or written reports. Then the researcher has to identify the most relevant segments of the text according to an organised scheme. Quite often, some categories emerge from the data, without having to apply a fix taxonomy. Sometimes, the researcher does the opposite: (s)he applies a predetermined classification.

In short, the two main types of techniques that can be identified in analysing qualitative data a the following:

a)      deriving a set of categories for dealing with text segments from the text itself (inductive procedure)

b)      applying a system of categories or predetermined classification to the data .

 

According to Tesch (1987),  there are some features that are common to all qualitative research analysis:

 

-          The analysis of qualitative data is systematic, but not rigid.

-          The main procedures used are comparison, a search for likeness and differences.

-          In order to be compared and contrasted, the raw data need to be summarised and condensed.

-          As a result of summarising and organising the information, some preliminary and tentative classifications of categories emerge.

-          The analysis is not the final phase of the research project. The results of  each analytical session point to other questions that need new data.

-          Qualitative analysis is a process that demands deep involvement on the part of the researcher.

-          Finally, there is not an exclusive and right way of analysing qualitative data. It is possible to analyse the information in different ways.

 

 

2.8.2      Analysing descriptive research data

 

Descriptive research is generally analysed by means of descriptive statistics. Some of the most common descriptive statistical procedures are: frequencies, central tendencies and variabilities.

a)      Frequencies are used to indicate how often a phenomenon occurs

b)      Central tendency measures provide information about the average and the typical behaviour of subjects.

-         The mean is the sum of all scores of all subjects in the group divided by the number of subjects.

-         The mode is the score which has been obtained by the largest number of subjects, i.e. the most frequent score in the group.

-         The median is the score which divides the group into two parts, so that half of the score are above it and half are below it.

c)      Variability provides information on the differences or spread of the behaviours. It indicates how homogeneous (or heterogeneous) the groups are. The most common variability measure is the standard deviation:  the higher the standard deviation, the more heterogeneous a group is. Another measure of variability used in statistical analyses is the variance, which is the standard deviation squared.

 

 

2.8.3      Correlational research data

 

Correlation techniques are used to explore existing relations between variables. For example, if we have data about the students’ achievement in the EFL class and in the L1 class, we can correlate data and study the relationship between achievement in L1 and in the FL class. If a positive correlation is obtained

it means that there is a close relationship between both variables. A negative correlation would indicate the opposite view.

Correlations  are indicated by means of the correlation coefficient, which ranges from – 1.00, indicating perfect negative correlation to 1.00, which indicates perfect positive correlation. The significant level obtained is very important since it relates directly to whether the null hypothesis is rejected or not. The conventional level of rejecting the null hypothesis is p < .05 or p < .01. When reporting correlations the researcher needs to specify all these data: the sample size (n) the correlations were based on, as well as the level of significance (p).

 

2.8.4      Multivariate research data

 

There are three well known multivariate procedures (see Seliger and Shohami, 1989:222-231)

 

1)      Multiple regression, which is used to examine the relationship and predictive power of independent variables. In the case of the relationship between L1 and L2, regression would indicate the prediction of L2 achievement under the influence of L1.

2)      Discriminant analysis indicates which combination of independent  variables distinguish between two or more categories of the dependent variable. For example, a researcher may want to study which combination of variables, L1, motivation, aptitude, etc. can best distinguish between two types of second language learners (males/females; learning in formal/informal contexts).

3)      Factor analysis. In this case the interrelationships between and among the variables of the data are examined in an attempt to find out how many independent dimensions can be identified in the data. Factor analysis is a procedure frequently used to validate language tests, for example, to check if the items of the Cultural tests really measure the cultural competence.

 

 

2.8.5      Experimental research data

 

When a control and experimental group are used in experimental research, other procedures are commonly used to compare results: The t-test, ANOVA and Chi-square.

 

At present , most of the data analysis techniques described in the previous sections can be performed with the computer. Some common statistical packages are: Statgraphics and SPSS (Statistical Package for Social Science).

 

 

2.9              CONCLUSION

 

In this brief overview on classroom observation and research, we have defined the main key concepts of the field and have presented a concise survey of the scope, methods and paradigms as well as their corresponding research techniques. We hope that our readers appreciate the variety of options they have if they intend to do classroom research (CL).

 

As we could see, CL on language teaching and learning has moved from the early simple attempts to identify “good” language teaching  towards the study of classroom interaction processes. This move has brought specialised researchers and teachers much nearer in order to study, sometimes in a co-operative way, what actually happens in the classroom and not to test or apply what other people think that should happen.

 

We have seen that , in instructed second language acquisition, what happens in the classroom is crucial if we want to understand how language learning takes place. We should not forget that the learning opportunities that learners have are confined to the classroom in most contexts. That is why the way teachers and learners interact determine the way that  the language is learnt. Learners do not seem to learn from the lesson plan but from the way it is implemented: from the input provided, the practice opportunities and the intake that they experience.

 

One basic goal of this unit has been to present several alternatives for classroom observation and analysis in order to study what happens in class and draw practical conclusions to improve the teaching and learning process and the final outcomes. Several models and procedures for CR have been introduced which involve varying attitudes on variables control and the intervention in the research study. We think that each method and paradigm has its place and it is useful for different purposes. The researcher must select the most appropriate approach according to the type of study that (s)he aims to carry out. The usefulness of particular research instruments depend on the purposes of the research, the limits of generalizability and other factors that we have mentioned in the previous pages.

 

We have given to observation a very high status, since it has often been considered the single most important component of classroom research. In this sense, classroom research is contextual research. It begins with detailed observation, recording and transcription. Such techniques include conversational analysis, linguistic variation, etc. They involve the use of intuitive judgements and decisions but also the establishment of a coherent research plan which inform about what is going to be studied  and how we aim to proceed (methodology).

 

Nevertheless, the most important conclusion may be to recognise the importance of classroom research as a basic tool for innovation in ELT. When classroom teachers practise research they use their knowledge and experience to explore specific classroom phenomena in order to gain a better understanding and find out their right meaning. In this sense, CR may also contribute to the classroom teacher professional development. This enquiring practices promote the teacher’s personal construction of his/her knowledge and help to improve the quality of classroom teaching and learning.

 

The consideration of teachers as researchers of their own practice has been fully discussed when describing the action research and reflection-in-action paradigms. These two paradigms advocate a new philosophy which gives teachers a more active role in the classroom when they reflect about classroom events and draw personal conclusions to solve the daily pedagogic problems, retreating from the standardised ready-made techniques supplied by official research.

 

But CR is not easy. We must admit that an enquiring attitude in education demands certain qualities from teachers:

-         A general background about education and, in our case, in ELT methodology.

-         An enquiring and diagnostic ability

-         Identifying the implied theories, approaches and paradigms that lay the foundations of the classroom techniques that are applied every day.

-         Designing strategies for future decision-making occasions.

-         A critic evaluation and reflection which is shared by other colleagues for the classroom research process to become co-operative.

 

We hope that the theoretical principles, models and project work that we have introduced in this unit also encourage our readers to become researcher of their own practice in order to know themselves better and improve the results of their  work.

 

 

2.10          FURTHER READING

 

ALLWRIGHT, D AND BAILEY, K. M. (1991): Focus on the language classroom: An introduction to classroom research for language teachers. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

VAN LIER, L. (1988): The Classroom and the Language Learner. London: Longman

CHAUDRON, C. (1988): Second Language Classrooms. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

WAJNRYB, R. (1992): Classroom Observation Tasks: A Resource Book for Language Teachers and Trainers. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

SELIGER, H. W.  (1989): Second Language Research Method. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

BROWN, J. D.  (1988): Understanding Research in Second Language Learning: A Teacher’s Guide to Statistics and Research Design. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

MALAMAH-THOMAS, A. (1987): Classroom Interaction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

ALLWRIGHT, D. (1988): Observation in the Language Classroom. London: Longman

McLAREN, N. , MADRID, D. (1996): A Handbook for TEFL. Alcoy: Marfil

MADRID, D., N. McLAREN (1995): Didactic Procedures for TEFL. Valladolid: La Calesa.